Forensic Psychology

Home > Other > Forensic Psychology > Page 95
Forensic Psychology Page 95

by Graham M Davies


  Brennan, T., Breitenbach, M., Dieterich, W., Salisbury, E. J., & Van Voorhis, P. (2012).Women’s pathways to serious and habitual crime: A person-centered analysis incorporating gender responsive factors. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 39, 1481–1508.

  Briere, J., & Jordan, C. E. (2009). Childhood maltreatment, intervening variables, and adult psychological difficulties in women: An overview. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 10, 375–388.

  Cernkovich, S. A., Lanctôt, N., & Giordano, P. C. (2008). Predicting adolescent and adult antisocial behavior among adjudicated delinquent females. Crime and Delinquency, 54, 3–33.

  Chamberlain, P. (2003). Treating chronic juvenile offenders: Advances made through the Oregon multidimensional treatment foster care model. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  Chesney-Lind, M., & Shelden, R.G. (2014). Girls, delinquency, and juvenile justice (4th ed.). Malden, MA: Wiley Blackwell.

  Claus, R. E., Orwin, R. G., Kissin, W., Krupski, A., Campbell, K., & Stark, K. (2007). Does gender-specific substance abuse treatment for women promote continuity of care? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 32, 27–39.

  Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2002). Evaluation of the first 3 years of the Fast Track prevention trial with children at high risk for adolescent conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 19–35.

  Cortoni, F., Babchishin, K. M., & Rat, C. (2016). The proportion of sexual offenders who are female is higher than thought: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior. OnlineFirst: DOI:10.1177/0093854816658923.

  Cortoni, F., Hanson, R. K., & Coache, M. E. (2010). The recidivism rates of female sexual offenders are low: A meta-analysis. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 22, 387–401.

  Covington, S. S., & Bloom, B. E. (2006). Gender responsive treatment and services in correctional settings. Women & Therapy, 29, 9–33.

  Daly, K. (1994). Gender, crime, and punishment. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Dodge, K .A., Dishion, T. J., & Lansford, J. E. (Eds.). (2006). Interventions and policies that aggregate deviant youth, and strategies to optimize outcomes. New York: Guilford Press.

  Dowden, C., & Andrews, D. A. (1999). What works for female offenders: A meta-analytic review. Crime & Delinquency, 45, 438–452.

  Fontaine, N., Carbonneau, R., Barker, E. D., Vitaro, F., Hébert, M., Côté, S. M., Nagin, D. S., Zoccolillo, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Girls’ hyperactivity and physical aggression during childhood and adjustment problems in early adulthood: A 15-year longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 320–328.

  Fontaine, N., Carbonneau, R., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2009). Research review: A critical review of studies on the developmental trajectories of antisocial behavior in females. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 363–385.

  Foster, E. M., Jones, D. E., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2005). The high costs of aggression: Public expenditures resulting from conduct disorder. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 1767–1772.

  Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Gobeil, R., Blanchette, K., & Stewart, L. (2016). A meta-analytic review of correctional interventions for women offenders gender-neutral versus gender-informed approaches. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 43, 301–322.

  Hanson, R. K., & Morton-Bourgon, K. E. (2005). The characteristics of persistent sexual offenders: A meta-analysis of recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 1154–63.

  Heller, K., Price, R. H., & Hogg, J. R. (1990). The role of social support in community and clinical interventions. In B. R. Sarason, I. G. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social support: An interactional view (pp. 482–507). New York: John Wiley.

  Hodgins, D. C., El-Guebaly, N., & Addington, J. (1997). Treatment of substance abusers: Single or mixed gender programs? Addiction, 92, 805–812.

  Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. (1997). Developmental and social influences on young girls’ early problem behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 95–113.

  King, E. A. (2017). Outcomes of trauma-informed interventions for incarcerated women: A review. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 61, 667–688.

  Lanctôt, N. (2010). La délinquance féminine: un caractère spécifique à nuancer. In M. LeBlanc & M. Cussion (Eds.), Traité de criminologie empirique du Québec (4th ed.) (pp. 273–303). Montréal (Québec): Les Presses de l’Université de Montréal.

  Lanctôt, N., & Le Blanc, M. (2002). Explaining deviance by adolescent females. Crime and Justice: A Review of Research, 29, 113–202.

  Leve, L. D., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (2005). Intervention outcomes for girls referred from juvenile justice: Effects on delinquency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73,1181–1185.

  Lipsey, M. W. (1992). The effect of treatment on juvenile delinquents: Results from meta-analysis. In F. Lösel & D. Bender (Eds.), Psychology and law: International perspectives (pp. 131–143). New York: Walter De Gruyter.

  Moffitt, T. E. (1993). “Life-course-persistent” and “adolescent-limited” antisocial behaviour: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.

  Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course-persistent and adolescent-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In B. B. Layey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile disorder (pp. 49–75). New York: Guilford Press.

  Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Sex differences in antisocial behaviour: Conduct disorder, delinquency, and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. New York: Cambridge University Press.

  Odgers, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., Poulton, R., Sears, M. R., Thompson, W. M., & Caspi, A. (2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 673–716.

  Olds, D., Eckenrode, J., Henderson, C. R. Jr, Kitzman, H., Powers, J., Cole, R., Sidora, K., Morris, P., Pettitt, L. M., & Luckey, D. (1997). Long-term effects of home visitation on maternal life course and child abuse and neglect. Jama: Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 637–643.

  Olds, D., Henderson, C. R. Jr, Cole, R., Eckenrode, J., Kitzman, H., Luckey, D., Pettitt, L., Sidora, K., Morris, P., & Powers, J. (1998). Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior. Jama: Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 1238–1244.

  Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach to family intervention (vol. 3). Eugene, OR: Castalia.

  Pepler, D. J., & Sedighdeilami, F. (1998). Aggressive girls in Canada. Hull, QC: Applied Research Branch, Strategic Policy, Human Resources Development Canada.

  Petitclerc, A., Gatti, U., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2013). Effects of juvenile court exposure on crime in young adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 291–297.

  Public Safety Canada. (2006, December). Corrections and conditional release statistical overview: Annual report 2006. Ottawa: Author.

  Rumgay, J. (2004). Living with paradox: Community supervision of women offenders. In G. McIvor (Ed.). Women Who Offend (pp. 99–125). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

  Rutter, M. (1996). Transitions and turning points in developmental psychopathology: As applied to the age span between childhood and mid-adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 603–626.

  Salisbury, E. J., & VanVoorhis, P. (2009). Gendered pathways: A quantitative investigation of women Probationers’ paths to incarceration. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 541–566.

  Saxena, P., Messina, N. P., & Grella, C. E. (2014). Who benefits from gender-responsive treatment? Accounting for abuse history on longitudinal outcomes for women in prison. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 41, 417–432.

  Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., De
Genna, N., Grunzeweig, N., Temcheff, C. E., Schwartzman, A. E., & Ledingham, J. (2004). When aggressive girls become mothers: Problems in parenting, health, and development of physical aggression among adolescent boys and girls. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 61–75.

  Silverthorn, P., & Frick, P. J. (1999). Developmental pathways to antisocial behavior: The delayed- onset pathway in girls. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 101–126.

  Simpson, S. S., Yahner, J. L., & Dugan, L. (2008). Understanding women’s pathways to jail: Analysing the lives of incarcerated women. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 41, 84–108.

  Smith-Lovin, L., & Brody, C. (1989). Interruptions in group discussions: The effects of gender and group composition, American Sociological Review, 54, 424–435.

  Thornberry, T. P. (2005). Explaining multiple patterns of offending across the life course and across generations. Annals, AAPSS, 602, 156–195.

  van Wormer, K. (2010). Working with female offenders: A gender sensitive approach. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.

  Verlaan, P., & Déry, M. (Eds.) (2006). Les conduites antisociales des filles: comprendre pour mieux agir. Québec (Qc): Presses de l’Université du Québec.

  Warner, J. A. (2012). Women and Crime: A reference handbook. Santa Barbara, CA: Abc-clio.

  Zoccolillo, M. (1992). Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety disorders. A review. Journal of the American Academic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 547–556.

  Zoccolillo, M., Romano, E., Joubert, D., Mazzarello, T., Côté, S., Boivin, M., Pérusse, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). The intergenerational transmission of aggression and antisocial behavior. In R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 353–375). New York: Guilford Press.

  NOTES

  1 It is important to note that trends in delinquent behaviour may vary across countries and years.

  2 Within developmental theory, a risk factor increases the likelihood of developing antisocial behaviour. This is not to be confused with risk factor for recidivism.

  3 A protective factor mitigates the risk associated with antisocial behaviour.

  4 Indirect aggression, relational aggression, and social aggression are similar concepts with respect to the types of manipulative acts they involve. They differ in their emphasis and how researchers using the three concepts have developed them (see Archer & Coyne, 2005).

  21 Interventions for Offenders with Intellectual Disabilities

  R. LINDSAY, WILLIAM L. TAYLOR JOHN AND AMANDA M. MICHIE

  CHAPTER OUTLINE

  21.1 INTRODUCTION

  21.2 THE PREVALENCE OF ID IN OFFENDER POPULATIONS

  21.3 ID AS A RISK FACTOR FOR OFFENDING

  21.4 ASSESSMENT OF OFFENDERS WITH ID 21.4.1 Assessment of Anger Motivated Offenders

  21.4.2 Assessment of Sexual Offenders

  21.4.3 Assessment of Arsonists (Fire-setters)

  21.4.4 Assessment of Risk for any Future Incidents

  21.5 INTERVENTIONS WITH OFFENDERS WITH ID 21.5.1 Treatment for Anger and Violence

  21.5.2 Controlled Studies of Anger Management Training (AMT)

  21.5.3 Treatment of Sexual Offenders with ID

  21.5.4 Outcome Studies Evaluating Sex Offender Interventions

  21.5.5 Treatment of Fire-setters

  21.6 AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS AND CRIME

  21.7 SUMMARY

  LEARNING OUTCOMES

  BY THE END OF THIS CHAPTER, YOU SHOULD BE ABLE TO:

  Understand the way in which psychological approaches to offenders with Intellectual and Developmental Disability (IDD) might differ from mainstream approaches

  Appreciate the principal research methods used to explore the way in which offenders with IDD respond to assessment and treatment

  Understand the implications of existing findings for the care and treatment of offenders with IDD.

  21.1 INTRODUCTION

  The fact that a subgroup of offenders is known to have significant intellectual limitations has, for many years, presented difficulties for the development of assessment, management and treatment services in the criminal justice system. Therefore, researchers and service planners have been interested in prevalence, characteristics of offenders with intellectual limitations, types of crimes and so on. Once characteristics and special needs have been established, procedures must be adapted so that individuals can engage meaningfully with the criminal justice system and other agencies. Finally, an important question is the extent to which services can reduce reoffending and enable individuals to return to community living. In a worst-case scenario, it may be that any particular subgroup of offenders is impervious to intervention aimed at rehabilitation and the prevention of reoffending.

  It will not be possible to review research on all these issues in this chapter and we will restrict ourselves to the effectiveness of assessment and treatment for offenders with intellectual disability (ID). We will evaluate the evidence on the psychometric properties of assessments for psychological variables related to offending. We will also evaluate the effectiveness of treatment of some main criminal behaviours, such as violence and sexual offending.

  21.2 THE PREVALENCE OF ID IN OFFENDER POPULATIONS

  Lindsay, Hastings, and Beech (2011) have noted three major methodological difficulties in studying the prevalence of offenders with ID in criminal populations. First studies have been conducted in a variety of settings including prisons (MacEachron, 1979), Probation Services (Mason & Murphy, 2002), and appearances at court (Vanny, Levy, Greenberg, & Hayes, 2009). Vanny et al. (2009) studied 250 individuals appealing in magistrates courts in New South Wales, Australia and, using an intellectual screening test, found that 10% fell into the category of ID while a further 20% fell into the category of borderline intelligence. On the other hand, MacEachron (1979) found prevalence rates of 0.6–2.3% in state prisons in Maine and Massachusetts USA. She concluded that the method used in the assessment made a significant difference to the rates found.

  Screening tools tend to find higher prevalence rates than full psychometric assessment since screening assessments are designed to be over-inclusive, identifying people for further more exhaustive assessment. A third source of variation is the inclusion criteria used, particularly if those considered to be functioning in the borderline intelligence range (IQ 70–79) are included. It can be seen in the Vanny et al. (2009) study that prevalence would triple from 10% to 30%, if people with borderline intelligence were included in those offenders with ID. Indeed, when low intelligence is considered in studies on offenders (e.g. Farrington, 2005), IQ under 85 is sometimes the criterion.

  A number of recent reports have arrived at similar findings. In a study on prisons in the UK, Hayes, Shackell, Mottram, and Lancaster (2007) used a full psychometric assessment and also assessed adaptive behaviour in 140 prisoners and found that 2.9% of their sample fell in the range of ID. Herrington (2009) used a screening assessment to re-evaluate 185 young adult male prisoners in a young offender’s institution. He reported 10% of prisoners fell into the ID range. A study in Norway (Søndenaa, Rasmussen, Palmstierna, & Nøttestad, 2008), on 143 inmates randomly chosen from the prison population, used a screening instrument to assess for intellectual disability. They reported that 10.8% of their sample fell into the category of ID. It is important to remember that screening tools are designed to be over-inclusive.

  The methodological differences between studies continue. Crocker, Cote, Toupin, and St-Onge (2007) attempted to assess 749 offenders in a pre-trial holding centre in Montréal. For a number of reasons including refusal to participate, administrative difficulties and technical problems, they were only able to assess 281 participants with three subscales locally standardised mental ability scale (equivalent to a screening test). They reported that 18.9% were in the probable ID range and a further 29.9% in the borderline range. This is an unusually high prevalence. On the other hand, Holland and Persson (2011) investigated al
l sentenced male prisoners released from Victorian prisons, Australia, from July 2003 until June 2006 (9,481 prisoners). Using the WAIS III to assess IQ and using a criterion of IQ less than 70, they found a prevalence rate of 1.3%, which is consistent with the general population.

  Clearly, there are huge methodological differences between these two studies, one assessing a proportion of those presenting in a pre-trial holding centre and the other assessing all prisoners in a state, one using an intelligence screening and the other using a full cognitive assessment. The method of assessment, the location of the sample and the extent of the sample are likely to be the source of the differences in findings between studies. In a systematic review of studies of prevalence in prison, Fazel, Xenitidis and Powell (2008) concluded that in studies that fulfilled their criteria for scientific integrity, typically, 0.5–1.5% of prisoners were diagnosed with ID. This figure falls at the lower end of the prevalence estimates but, they concluded, still represents a highly significant number of individuals with ID in prisons and has implications for policy and practice.

  These methodological differences also are likely to be the source of differences found between studies in characteristics of offenders with ID. Walker and McCabe (1973) reported a high prevalence of sexual offences and arson in this client group. Lunsky, Gracey, Koegl, Bradley, Durbin, and Raina (2011) also found a higher rate of fire-setting and property destruction in offenders with ID held in psychiatric institutions when compared to those without ID. Hogue et al. (2006) reviewed the number of characteristics of offenders with ID across community, the medium/secure and high secure settings. They found that the rates of arson as an index offence depended on the setting with low rates in the community sample (2.9%) and higher rates in the medium secure sample (21.4%). This suggested that while rates of arson in offenders with ID are consistent with mainstream offenders, there may be a tendency to refer arsonists with ID to medium secure psychiatric settings, such as that found in the Lunsky et al. (2011) study, raising the prevalence in this setting.

 

‹ Prev