Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Home > Other > Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions > Page 25
Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions Page 25

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  did not ask attorneys and judges to predict the outcomes of laboratory experi-

  ments. Instead, we examined their courtroom behaviors (attorneys ’ arguments

  and judges ’ instructions) and converted the apparent logic underlying these

  behaviors into specifi c predictions. That is, people who engage in the described

  courtroom behaviors should make the following predictions about the out-

  comes of controlled, laboratory experiments. We refer to the implicit theory

  that underlies these courtroom arguments and jury instructions as the

  126

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  ‘ Courtroom Theory ’ of memory. What, specifi cally, are the predictions of the

  Courtroom Theory?

  1. Consistency of recollection is a direct indicator of the quality of a witness ’ s

  memory. Therefore, factors that infl uence consistency of recollection

  should have the same effect on accuracy of recollection. Experimental

  factors that increase (decrease) consistency should increase (decrease)

  accuracy and vice versa. No experimental factors should dissociate (have

  different effects on) consistency and accuracy.

  2. Contradictory statements should be inaccurate – at least as compared to

  consistent statements.

  3. Witnesses who make many contradictory statements should be consider-

  ably less accurate overall (across their entire testimony) than witnesses who

  make few or no contradictory statements. Statistically, there should be a

  strong, negative correlation between amount of inconsistency in a wit-

  ness ’ s testimony and the overall accuracy of the witness ’ s testimony.

  4. Reminiscence should occur infrequently. Moreover, explaining reminis-

  cence requires an extraordinary (non - cognitive) mechanism, such as police

  informing witnesses about crime details.

  5. Reminiscent statements should be inaccurate – at least as compared to

  consistent statements.

  6. Witnesses who make many reminiscent statements should be considerably

  less accurate overall (across their entire testimony) than witnesses who

  make few or no reminiscent statements. Statistically, there should be a

  strong, negative correlation between amount of reminiscence in a witness ’ s

  testimony and the overall accuracy of the witness ’ s testimony.

  As opposed to the Courtroom Theory of memory, how would cognitive

  psychology account for these witness behaviors?

  Cognitive Theory

  Two principles of cognitive theory are responsible for the major distinctions

  between the predictions of the Courtroom Theory and Cognitive Theory: the

  importance of retrieval processes; and the independence of components. We

  describe these principles briefl y.

  Retrieval p rocesses

  Recollection refl ects not only the contents of the memory store but also the

  process of retrieval (Tulving, 1983 ). If the retrieval processes applied on two

  occasions differ, the recollections will differ, even if the contents of memory

  do not change. The retrieval process is partially determined by the specifi c

  The Relation between Consistency and Accuracy of Eyewitness Testimony 127

  question that is asked. Thus, if the question that is posed to a witness changes

  from one interview to another, the witness ’ s recollections may change. In

  general, the more different are the retrieval cues (questions) across interviews,

  the more dissimilar will be the recollections on the two interviews. Reminiscence

  may occur, therefore, if a retrieval cue is present on the second interview, but

  not on the fi rst interview. The amount of reminiscence should refl ect the

  amount of dissimilarity between the retrieval cues (questions) on two

  interviews.

  Independence of c omponents

  Complex events are made up of many components, each of which is processed

  somewhat independently of the others (Fisher, Phillips & Krioukova, 2000 ;

  Mitchell, Haw & Fisher, 2003 ). Therefore, if a witness fails to recall one

  component of a crime, or even if she misperceives or mistakenly recalls one

  component of the crime, she may still perceive or correctly recall other com-

  ponents of the crime.

  Based on these two principles, cognitive psychology predicts the following

  behaviours by witnesses who are interviewed repeatedly:

  1. Some mental processes underpin measures of both consistency and accu-

  racy. For instance, the quality of the memory trace should infl uence both

  consistency and accuracy. Better encoded events should be recalled more

  consistently and also more accurately than poorly encoded events. By

  comparison, other mental processes do not underpin both consistency

  and accuracy: Either they have opposite effects on consistency and

  accuracy or they infl uence one measure but not the other. For instance,

  the similarity of the retrieval cues used across two interviews should infl u-

  ence consistency of recollection, but not accuracy of recollection. We

  should, therefore, expect that some experimental manipulations will have

  similar effects on consistency and accuracy, whereas other manipulations

  will have different effects on consistency and accuracy (experimental

  dissociation).

  2. As noted, better encoded events should be recalled more consistently and

  more accurately than poorly encoded events. We should also expect that

  encouraging witnesses to guess will lead to less consistent and less accurate

  responses than instructing witnesses to be certain before volunteering a

  response. Both of these propositions predict that consistent recollections

  will be more accurate than inconsistent recollections.

  3. Each component of a complex event is processed independently of the

  other components. Therefore, accuracy of recalling some components of

  a complex event may not necessarily predict how accurately witnesses recall

  the other components. Witnesses who make many contradictory state-

  ments may be inaccurate on those specifi c statements; however, they may

  be accurate on the rest of their testimony. That is, the correlation between

  128

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  amount of inconsistency and overall accuracy of a witness ’ s testimony may

  be relatively weak.

  4. Reminiscence should be a common experience. Furthermore, the amount

  of reminiscence should be explained easily by conventional cognitive

  theory, which relates reminiscence to changes in retrieval cues from one

  interview to another.

  5. Reminiscent statements may or may not be accurate. Whether reminiscent

  responses are accurate or not depends on a variety of factors, such as the

  nature of the question that is asked: Open - ended questions or probes (e.g.,

  Describe his face) should yield more accurate responses than closed ques-

  tions (e.g., What color were his eyes?).

  6. For the same reason as indicated in point 3 above (the independence of

  components), witnesses who make many reminiscent statements should

  not necessarily be inaccurate on the other (non - reminiscent) statements

  of their testimony. The correlation between amount of reminiscence and

  overall accuracy may be relatively we
ak.

  Experimental t esting

  We report an overview of the results from 19 experiments to assess the predic-

  tions of the Courtroom and Cognitive theories. Each of the experiments

  conformed to the following general procedure. Witnesses (typically college

  students, but the same patterns of results also obtained for others) either

  watched a videotape of a simulated crime (robbery or homicide) or observed

  a live, innocuous event or a staged confrontation between two people. The

  witnesses were then tested formally (paper

  - and - pencil test) or, as in most

  experiments, participated in face - to - face interviews to assess their memories of

  the event. Most of the witnesses were tested twice. The tests or interviews

  occurred either shortly after observing the event (within 30 minutes) or after

  a delay of up to two weeks. The interview questions or probes were either

  open - ended (e.g., Describe the robber) or were closed. There were three kinds

  of closed questions: cued recall (e.g., What color were the robber ’ s eyes?),

  multiple choice (What color was the robber

  ’ s eyes: green, blue, black, or

  brown?) and True/False (The robber ’ s eyes were green: true or false?). The

  witnesses were sometimes encouraged to be very certain before volunteering

  an answer, sometimes encouraged to guess, and sometimes not given any

  explicit instructions about certainty.

  We compared the witness statements across the two interviews and cate-

  gorized them as one of four types: Consistent (same answer at Time 1 and

  Time 2, e.g., robber was a white male at Time 1, and robber was a white male

  at Time 2), contradiction (contradictory answers at Time 1 ( clean - shaven ) and

  Time 2 ( bearded) ), reminiscent (no answer at Time 1, but witness provides

  an answer at Time 2 ( red shirt ), and forgotten (witness provides an answer at

  Time 1 ( baseball hat ) but does not answer at Time 2. We then calculated the

  The Relation between Consistency and Accuracy of Eyewitness Testimony 129

  accuracy of each of the four response categories (consistent, contradiction,

  reminiscent, and forgotten) in addition to the accuracy of the entire testi-

  mony. Accuracy was calculated by dividing the number of correct statements

  by the total number of statements. For instance, if a witness made eight

  correct statements out of ten total statements, then her accuracy rate was 0.8

  (8/10).

  The results of these experiments are organized around the predictions of

  the ‘ Courtroom ’ and ‘ Cognitive ’ Theories.

  Experimental d issociations: c ommon v ersus u nique m ental p rocesses. Some

  experimental manipulations had the same effects on consistency and accuracy

  of testimony, whereas other manipulations had different effects on the two

  measures. When witnesses were instructed to be certain before volunteering

  answers, they were both more consistent and more accurate than when

  instructed to guess if unsure (Phillips, Fisher & Krioukova, 1999 ). Varying

  the format of the question (open - ended vs. closed) also had parallel effects on

  consistency and accuracy: Open

  - ended questions yielded more consistent

  responses and also more accurate answers than closed questions (Fisher &

  Patterson, 2004 ). Other manipulations, however, had different, and some-

  times opposite, effects on consistency and accuracy of recollection. Specifi cally,

  witnesses were less consistent but more accurate when tested shortly after the

  critical event (within 30 minutes) than when tested after two weeks (Fisher,

  Schreiber, Burguera & Alvarez, 2003 ). That is, delaying the tests increased

  consistency but decreased accuracy. This experimental dissociation suggests

  that consistency and accuracy may refl ect different underlying mechanisms

  (Tulving, 1985 ), in opposition to the Courtroom Theory.

  Accuracy of c ontradictions. In all of our experiments, the accuracy rate of

  contradictory answers was low (Brock, Fisher

  & Cutler,

  1999 ; Fisher

  &

  Patterson, 2004 ; Gilbert & Fisher, 2006 ; ). For instance, in Gilbert & Fisher,

  the accuracy rate of contradictory statements was only 0.49; by comparison,

  the accuracy rate of consistent answers was almost perfect (0.95). Both the

  Courtroom and Cognitive theories correctly predicted the low accuracy of

  contradictory statements.

  Contradiction as a p redictor of o verall a ccuracy. Although contradictory

  statements were considerably less accurate than consistent statements, wit-

  nesses who made many contradictory statements were not much less accurate

  overall (all of the statements in their entire testimony) than were witnesses

  who made only a few contradictory statements. Witnesses were scored in terms

  of the accuracy of their overall testimony and the proportion of all statements

  that were contradictory (typically, this proportion is relatively small, as wit-

  nesses who take their task seriously rarely make more than a few contradic-

  tions). Across the various conditions of the experiments, the correlations

  130

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  between the proportion of contradictory statements and the accuracy of the

  entire testimony was relatively low (the Pearson correlation coeffi cient was

  generally between 0.00 and 0.35) (Fisher & Cutler, 1995 ; Brewer et al ., 1999 ;

  Fisher & Patterson, 2004 ; Gilbert & Fisher, 2006 ). The fi nding that contra-

  dictions are poor predictors of witnesses ’ overall testimonial accuracy is in

  direct violation of the ‘ Courtroom ’ theory.

  Reminiscence: f requency and e xtraordinary e xplanations. In our studies,

  and also those of other researchers (e.g., Scrivner & Safer, 1988 ), reminiscence

  was a common phenomenon (see Payne, 1987 , for a review). In Gilbert &

  Fisher (2006) , 98% of witnesses who were tested twice (189 of 192) made at

  least two reminiscent recollections, hardly a rare phenomenon, as suggested

  by the Courtroom Theory. Furthermore, the number of reminiscent state-

  ments a witness made was highly related to the dissimilarity of the questions

  (retrieval cues) that were asked on the two tests. When the retrieval cues

  changed from Test 1 to Test 2, witnesses made almost twice as many reminis-

  cent statements (10.1) as when the same cues were given on the two tests

  (6.1). As this fi nding is compatible with the Cognitive Theory, one need not

  postulate extraordinary mechanisms to account for reminiscence, as the

  Courtroom Theory suggests.

  Accuracy of

  r eminiscence. Reminiscent statements varied in accuracy

  across studies, from a low of 0.66 (Brock et al ., 1999 ) to a high of 0.87

  (Gilbert & Fisher, 2006 ). That reminiscent statements can be very accurate

  violates the dire predictions of the Courtroom Theory, which assumes refl ex-

  ively that reminiscence is problematic (see also LaRooy, Pipe

  & Murray,

  2005 ).

  Although reminiscent statements were often accurate, they were not as

  accurate as either consistent or forgotten statements. In Gilbert

  & Fisher

  (2006) , for example, the accuracy rates for consistent, forgotten
, and reminis-

  cent, statements were, respectively, 0.95, 0.93, and 0.87 (see also Brock et al .,

  1999 ; Fisher & Patterson, 2004 , for similar patterns). Reminiscent statements,

  however, were much more accurate than were contradictions (0.49). At the

  very least, then, we should distinguish between different kinds of inconsistency

  and pay most attention to direct contradictions.

  Reminiscence as a p redictor of o verall a ccuracy. The prevalence of remi-

  niscent statements was not predictive of overall accuracy. Witnesses who made

  more reminiscent statements were only minimally, and non - signifi cantly, less

  accurate than witnesses who made fewer reminiscent statements. In Gilbert &

  Fisher (2006) the Pearson correlation coeffi cient between proportion of remi-

  niscent statements and overall accuracy was − 0.05. This correlation was uni-

  formly low across the two tests: Proportion of reminiscent statements was

  non - signifi cantly correlated with overall accuracy at Test 1 (0.03) and also at

  The Relation between Consistency and Accuracy of Eyewitness Testimony 131

  Test 2 ( − 0.14). Similar patterns of non - signifi cant correlations were also found

  in Fisher & Patterson (2006) and Gordon & Follmer (1994) .

  Relative to consistent recollections, which are by far the most common

  category of response, reminiscent responses are relatively infrequent (approxi-

  mately 22% of all responses in Gilbert & Fisher, 2006 ) and contradictions are

  very rare (less than 1%). To increase the reliability of these infrequent events,

  we therefore combined contradictory and reminiscent statements into one

  score to determine if these ‘ troublesome ’ inconsistencies, when aggregated,

  were more predictive of overall accuracy. Specifi cally, we examined whether

  the amount of inconsistency was related to the accuracy of consistent items.

  The number of inconsistencies (contradictions and reminiscences) was not at

  all predictive of the accuracy of consistent items (correlation coeffi cient = − 0.06).

  Even when we compared the most discrepant witnesses (those who made

  12 – 18 inconsistencies) to the most consistent witnesses (0 – 6 inconsistencies),

  the accuracy rates of consistent items did not differ, 0.94 vs. 0.96, respectively.

  No matter how we scored the data, there was no evidence to support the

  Courtroom Theory that reminiscence is predictive of inaccuracy of the overall

 

‹ Prev