Book Read Free

Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Page 28

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  cated these fi ndings with four and fi ve year olds. Importantly, their research

  established that just two CI instructions (

  contest reinstatement and

  report

  everything ) in combination reduced children

  ’ s reporting of misinformation

  during the interview and later memory tests. We shall return to this study

  below.

  Do CI s e nhance o lder w itnesses ’ r ecollections?

  Older adults represent a special group of witnesses. In many countries, growing

  numbers of older adults are remaining active in the community. Hence, it is

  likely that some will witness or be a victim of a crime. Moreover, physical and

  The Cognitive Interview

  143

  emotional abuse of the elderly is being reported with increasing frequency

  (Action on Elder Abuse, 2004 ) with those over the age of 75 years being

  particularly vulnerable. Obtaining reliable eyewitness testimony from older

  witnesses has now become a key concern to policy - makers and professionals,

  yet only a small number of laboratory studies have evaluated the reliability

  and accuracy of older adults ’ recollections in an eyewitness context. Findings

  from these studies reveal that memory recall is less complete and less accu-

  rate in comparison to young adults, whether the witnessed event is a slide

  show (Yarmey & Kent, 1980 ), a short fi lm (List, 1986 ) or a staged event

  (Yarmey, 1993 ). Such age differences are reported when memory is tested

  immediately, minutes or days after the witnessed event (Brimacombe, Quinton,

  Nance & Garrioch, 1997 ; see Mueller - Johnson & Ceci, 2007 , for an excellent

  review).

  A handful of studies have evaluated the effectiveness of CI techniques with

  older adults. In an early study, young (18 – 35 years) and old (65 – 80 years)

  adults were shown a short fi lm of a staged robbery (Mello & Fisher, 1996 ).

  Half an hour later, both groups were given a regular police interview, a full

  CI or a modifi ed CI (omitted change perspective ). An unexpected age pattern

  was found in that when given a full CI older adults provided more correct

  information than young adults. Research by McMahon

  (2000) evaluated

  whether a full CI would increase correct recall of a fi lm of a simulated crime.

  Young (18 – 50 years) and old (60 – 88 years) adults were interviewed 30 minutes

  after viewing the fi lm. As expected, the younger adults recollected more correct

  information than the older adults, but only in the control interview (see also

  Isingrini, Vazou & Leroy, 1995 ). In Rose, Bull & Vrij ’ s (2003) study, young

  (18 – 31 years) and old (59 – 84 years) adults viewed a short fi lm of a staged

  robbery. Thirty minutes later all participants were presented with line - up iden-

  tifi cation tasks. Whilst the older adults made more incorrect identifi cations

  than the young adults, no effects of mental or physical context reinstatement

  instructions on performance were found. In a second recent line - up study with

  young (16 – 30 years) and old (64 – 86 years) adults, Wilcock, Bull & Vrij (2007)

  reported that context reinstatement increased correct rejections in target

  absent line - ups but only in the old adults.

  More recently, two studies by Wright and Holliday provided evidence that

  CI protocols can increase correct recollections in elderly witnesses. Wright &

  Holliday (2007a) evaluated older witnesses ’ recall of a short fi lm using full CI,

  a MCI (omitting change perspective instruction) or a control interview. The

  full CI increased correct recall by 20% for young adults (aged 17 – 31 years),

  27% for young - old adults (aged 60 – 74 years), and 18% for old - old adults (aged

  75 – 95 years), while the MCI increased correct recall by 14% for young adults,

  17% for young - old adults and 15% for old - old adults. In the second study,

  Wright

  & Holliday

  (2007b) compared the recollections of old adults

  (aged 75 – 96 years) who displayed evidence of cognitive impairments on the

  Mini - Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein, Folstein

  & McHugh,

  144

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  1975 ) with old adults without cognitive impairments on MMSE. Old adults

  with a low MMSE score recalled fewer correct details and were less accu-

  rate than those with high scores. Importantly, high and low MMSE old adult

  groups reported substantially more correct information about Action, Person,

  Object and Surrounding details with a MCI than with a control interview.

  Do CI t echniques i mprove i ntellectually d isabled

  w itnesses ’ r ecollections?

  Individuals with intellectual disabilities (ID) are considered vulnerable

  witnesses. Research with these adults reports that they are slower than

  typically developed adults to encode, store and retrieve details of an event

  (Milne & Bull, 2001 ). Adults with IDs have been considered by courts to be

  unreliable witnesses ( ibid. ). Yet the information that ID adults do recollect is

  just as accurate as that of other adult witnesses. However, ID adults do report

  fewer details of a witnessed event than other adults (Perlman, Ericsson, Esses

  & Isaacs, 1994 ; Milne & Bull, 2001 ). Given that these witnesses have been

  found to be highly suggestible (Cardone & Dent, 1996 ; Milne, Clare & Bull,

  1999 ), it is of major importance that ID adults, like other groups of vulnerable

  witnesses, be questioned appropriately and non

  - suggestively. Research has

  demonstrated that ID adults are particularly susceptible to the negative effects

  of social demand factors. Kebbell & Hatton (1999) reviewed the research

  literature and reported that ID adults are likely to say ‘ yes ’ to questions irre-

  spective of the content of such questions. ID adults are more likely than other

  adults to fabricate and change answers in response to the interviewer ’ ques-

  tions (Clare & Gudjonsson, 1993 ; Ternes & Yuille, 2008 ). They are also

  highly suggestible (Milne, Clare & Bull, 2002 ; Ternes & Yuille, 2008 ).

  The body of research in which CI protocols have been evaluated with cog-

  nitively impaired adults is sparse. In the fi rst such study, Brown & Geiselman

  (1990) reported that ID adults recalled fewer correct details than other adults.

  This effect did not vary by type of interview (whether CI or control). ID adults

  also reported more confabulated details during a CI than other adults. In

  Milne, Clare & Bull ’ s (1999) study, adults with and without mild ID watched

  a short fi lm of an accident and were interviewed the next day with either a CI

  or a structured (control) interview (K ö hnken, 1993 ). An increase in correct

  details was found for those given a CI. However, ID adults given a CI reported

  more confabulated details than ID adults given a control interview (Milne &

  Bull, 2001 ). More recently, CI principles have been tested on elderly adults

  with dementia (Wright & Holliday, 2007b ). Adults (75 – 96 years) with and

  without cognitive impairments were given a modifi ed CI (omit CP), or a full

  CI, or a control interview following viewing a short fi lm. Cognitive - impaired

  adults remembered fewer c
orrect details than non - impaired adults. Nonetheless,

  both types of CIs enhanced recollections of both impaired and non - impaired

  elderly adults, although impaired participants had particular diffi culties with

  The Cognitive Interview

  145

  the CP instruction. Clearly, more research is needed before fi rm conclusions

  can be drawn about the benefi ts (or not) of using CI protocols with these

  vulnerable individuals.

  The research literature in which CI protocols have been evaluated with

  intellectually disabled (ID) children is extremely small. At the time of writing,

  all this work has been conducted in the UK by Milne and Bull. Several years

  ago, Milne & Bull (2001) pointed out in a review article that the recollections

  of ID children for events they have experienced or witnessed are typically less

  complete than those of children without ID (much like ID adults). Therefore,

  they argued ( ibid .) that it is crucial that researchers evaluate protocols that

  have the potential to enhance witness testimony, such as the CI. Milne & Bull

  (1996) gave ID children a CI or a control interview similar to the MOGP

  interview (Home Offi ce, 1992 ) following a witnessed event. Children given a

  CI recollected a greater number of correct details in comparison to children

  given a control interview. Signifi cantly, a CI did not lead to increases in report-

  ing of incorrect and confabulated details.

  How e ffective a re CI p rotocols in the fi eld?

  The revised CI has proved benefi cial when evaluated with adult witnesses and

  victims of real crime. In the fi rst study (Fisher, Geiselman & Amador, 1989 ),

  police detectives from the Metro - Dade Police Department, Miami conducted

  interviews before and after four hours ’ training in the revised CI protocols.

  Importantly, the amount of information obtained from witnesses by police

  after training increased substantially (47%). A second fi eld study was conducted

  in the UK by George (1991, cited in George & Clifford, 1996 ). Police who

  had been trained in the CI protocols interviewed young adults after they had

  witnessed a staged argument. Recall of correct details was substantially higher

  in CIs than in control interviews, without a concomitant increase in reporting

  of incorrect information. Notably, the size of this recall advantage is remark-

  ably similar to fi ndings from laboratory studies (Fisher & Schreiber, 2007 ).

  More recently, Fisher & Castano (2008) reported that CI protocols have been

  used effectively in the USA in several police investigations, including child

  sexual abuse, kidnapping, a bombing and a murder enquiry. In the last case,

  a CI was used with a female who had witnessed a murder 33 years earlier when

  she was fi ve years old.

  Promising a pplications of the CI p rotocols

  Do the a dvantages of a CI r emain with a s hortened v ersion?

  For practitioners, shortened interview protocols that facilitate accurate recol-

  lections are important given the potential problems surrounding interviewing

  146

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  vulnerable witnesses (limited attention span, shorter memory) and pressures

  on police and other professionals to obtain maximum information as soon

  as possible after a crime. Wright & Holliday (2005) conducted a study in

  which police offi cers from a number of constabularies in the UK responded

  to a questionnaire. Police who were reluctant to use CI techniques stated that

  such protocols are far too long and mentally demanding to be of practical use

  when interviewing elderly witnesses and victims. Davis, McMahon

  &

  Greenwood

  (2005) evaluated a shortened version of a CI (CRI and RE

  instructions). College students viewed a short fi lm of a staged crime and were

  subsequently interviewed with a full CI, a short CI or a control interview.

  Importantly, correct recollection of the fi lm details was higher in the full CI

  and the short CI (equally) than in a control interview condition. Dando,

  Wilcock & Milne (2009) , using college students, investigated two variations

  of context reinstatement instructions: mental context reinstatement (MRI) as

  used in cognitive interviews, and a shortened form they called ‘ sketch rein-

  statement of context ’ (MRC). The sketch MRC condition produced more

  accurate recollections of a fi lmed crime than the MRI and control interview

  conditions.

  Holliday & Albon (2004) conducted a comprehensive study that aimed

  to develop a developmentally appropriate shortened CI for young children

  which minimized suggestibility. The rationale behind this research was

  Holliday ’ s (2003b) fi ndings that a Modifi ed CI (MCI) reduced children ’ s

  acceptance and subsequent reporting of suggestions. Given the short attention

  span of very young children, a shortened version of the MCI would be par-

  ticularly useful. Children viewed a short fi lm followed by exposure to a number

  of misleading suggestions. Children were then administered one of six inter-

  view protocols:

  • a control interview (structured interview; K ö hnken, 1993 );

  • a full CI (FCI) containing the CRI, RE, CO and CP instructions;

  • a Modifi ed CI, which was identical to the FCI except CP was omitted;

  • an Enhanced Rapport MCI (ERMCI), which was identical to the MCI

  except for an additional fi ve minutes of rapport - building in which children

  described a favourite game;

  • a RE and CO interview, which was the same as the MCI except that CRI

  was omitted;

  • a RE and CRI interview which resembled the MCI except that CO was

  omitted.

  These interview protocols are presented in Table 9.1 .

  In terms of the quality of communication (e.g. rapport

  - building, active

  listening) and questioning methods, SIs and CIs used in Holliday & Albon ’ s

  (2004) research are identical

  – each interview protocol employs a phased

  The Cognitive Interview

  147

  Table 9.1: Child interview protocols tested in Holliday & Albon ’ s (2004) research

  Cognitive interviews

  Control interview

  1. Rapport - building phase

  1. Rapport - building phase

  Chat about general interests the child

  Chat about General interests of

  (e.g., pets, football)

  the child (e.g. of pets, football)

  Describe their favourite game 1

  Explain aims/rules of interview a

  Explain aims/rules of interview a

  2. Free recall phase

  2. Free recall phase

  a. Context reinstatement 1,2,3,5

  b. Report everything 1,2,3,4,5

  c. Change order 1,2,3,4

  d. Change perspective 3

  e. Free recall report request a

  Free recall report request a

  3. Questioning phase

  3. Questioning phase

  Details reported in Free Recall Phase

  Details reported in Free Recall

  are used as the bases of open - ended

  Phase are used as the bases of

  and specifi c and questions in this

  open - ended specifi c questions in

  phase. a

  thi
s phase. a

  4. Closure a

  4. Closure a

  Notes :

  a All interviews.

  1. Enhanced rapport CI. 2. MCI. 3. Full CI. 4. Report everything and change order interview.

  5. Report everything and context reinstatement interview.

  approach proceeding from free recall to open, to closed, to specifi c

  questions.

  The CIs produced more correct recollections than a control interview.

  As has been reported previously, CIs enhanced children

  ’ s recall of

  person, action and object information (cf. Milne & Bull, 2002; 2003 ; Holli-

  day,

  2003a, 2003b

  ), without an accompanying increase in reporting of

  incorrect or confabulated details (cf. McCauley & Fisher, 1996 ; Granhag &

  Spjut,

  2001 ; Holliday,

  2003a; 2003b

  ). The fi nding that young children

  ’ s

  recall of person details can be improved with CI instructions is very important

  given that investigative interviews necessarily require accurate witness

  descriptions.

  In earlier CI research, concerns were raised that the CO and CP mnemon-

  ics might encourage young children to confabulate (e.g. Ceci, Bruck & Battin,

  2000 ; Memon & K ö hnken, 1992 ; Saywitz et al ., 1992 ), although others (e.g.

  Milne & Bull, 2002 ; Holliday, 2003a; 2003b ) have reported no increase in

  148

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  reporting of incorrect or confabulated details in young children. The CO

  mnemonic was included in MCI, FCI, Enhanced Rapport Modifi ed Cognitive

  Interview (ERMCI), and RE and CO interviews because research has shown

  that children can manage this instruction when it is accompanied by frequent

  prompts (Milne & Bull, 2002 ; Holliday, 2003a; 2003b ). Given the concerns

  that correct implementation of the CP instruction may be beyond the capabili-

  ties of very young children (Geiselman

  & Padilla,

  1988 ; Newcombe

  &

  Huttenlocher, 1992 ), and that it is unpopular with police offi cers in England

  (Memon & Stevenage, 1996 ), this instruction was only included in a FCI with

  the specifi c aim of evaluating its effectiveness with young children. However,

  as reported by others (e.g. Milne & Bull, 2002 ; Holliday, 2003a; 2003b )

  children had little diffi culty in using the CO mnemonic, demonstrating the

 

‹ Prev