Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Home > Other > Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions > Page 37
Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions Page 37

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  190

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  video recording of the moment of the crash on television. Whilst it has never

  been established whether such a fi lm exists, it has certainly never been shown

  on television. Nevertheless, 44% of Ost et al. ’ s participants claimed to have

  seen it. Furthermore, many participants even went as far as to say on which

  television channel they had seen it. Ost et al. found that participants who

  scored higher on a measure of ‘ eagerness to please ’ (the self - monitoring scale;

  Snyder, 1974 ) were more like to claim to have seen the fi lm than participants

  who scored lower on this measure.

  In a replication of the Crombag et al. study, Granhag, Str ö mwall & Billings

  (2003) asked participants whether they had seen a (nonexistent) fi lm of the

  sinking of the Estonia ferry. Again they found that 52% of their participants

  claimed to have seen the fi lm. Granhag et al. (2003) were interested in whether

  these false reports were susceptible to social infl uence. Thus, in a novel twist,

  they had a confederate present when the participants completed the question-

  naire. This confederate either claimed to have seen the nonexistent fi lm or

  claimed not to have seen it. Granhag

  et al. found that participants either

  increased, or decreased, their levels of false reporting in line with the social

  infl uence exerted by the confederate. When the confederate claimed to have

  seen the fi lm, the number of participants also claiming to have seen it increased,

  and vice versa (see Ost, Hogbin & Granhag, 2006 , for a replication).

  The ‘ crashing memory ’ effect appears to be robust, with between 36% and

  66% of participants in any given study claiming to have seen the nonexistent

  video footage (Jelicic, Smeets, Peters, Candel & Merckelbach, 2006 ; Wilson

  & French, 2006 ; but see Jelicic, Smeets, Candel, van Suijdam & Merckelbach,

  2006 ). These rates, however, can be reduced by phrasing the misleading ques-

  tion in an unambiguous rather than ambiguous manner (i.e., asking about ‘ a ’

  fi lm, rather than ‘ the ’ fi lm; Smeets et al. , 2006 ).

  Parental m isinformation m ethod

  The studies mentioned above show that some individuals will, when misled

  by subtle changes in wording, or by a confederate, come to report that they

  remember events, or details of events, that they did not witness. However, it

  is diffi cult to generalize these fi ndings to cases of allegedly false or recovered

  memories of childhood events. The events in the Crombag et al. , Ost et al.

  and Granhag et al. studies were all relatively recent and had occurred whilst

  the participants in their studies were adults. In contrast, most delayed claims

  of childhood abuse concern events that, by defi nition, occurred many years

  beforehand (see Pendergrast, 1996 ). Can individuals be misled to report false

  events from their childhood?

  Loftus & Coan (cited in Loftus & Pickrell, 1995 ) describe a study in which

  a 14 - year - old boy, Chris, was asked to recall details over fi ve days regarding

  four events involving family members. One of the events was false and other

  three were true (as verifi ed by the family). Chris was interviewed in the pres-

  ence of a sibling (a confederate of the investigators) about these events. The

  Recovered Memories

  191

  sibling provided verbal corroboration that all the events (including the false

  event) had taken place. Over time Chris began to report more about the four

  events, even rating the false event (becoming lost in a shopping mall as a child)

  as more likely to have occurred than all but one of the three true events.

  Loftus & Pickrell (1995) replicated this effect with a larger sample of under-

  graduate students. In this study participants were asked to complete a booklet

  concerning four events, the third of which was false (becoming lost in a shop-

  ping mall). Parents who confi rmed that their child had never become lost in

  a shopping mall as a child also provided details of the three real events.

  Participants were interviewed three times over three weeks and also asked,

  between interviews, to write down in their booklets anything that came to

  mind about the events. Loftus & Pickrell (1995) found that after three weeks

  six out of 24 participants (25%) erroneously believed part or all of the false

  event. However, as Pezdek, Finger & Hodge (1997) argue, becoming lost in

  a shopping mall is a fairly common occurrence for which many people would

  have a ‘ script ’ (or ‘ schema ’ ). Pezdek et al. contend that it would be relatively

  easy for an individual to create a convincing report of an event like this,

  although as Loftus ( 1997b : 180) argues, the important point is not that par-

  ticipants might be able to construct a general narrative of such an event, but

  that they report specifi c details suggested by the experimenter. In these studies

  participants ‘ were not asked about ANY experience of being lost. They were

  asked to remember being lost around the age of fi ve, in a particular location,

  with particular people present, being frightened, and ultimately being rescued

  by an elderly person ’ ( ibid. ) In fact, Pezdek et al. (1997) demonstrated that

  an event that was lower in plausibility and for which participants were less

  likely to have ‘ script - relevant ’ knowledge (in this case receiving an enema as a

  child) was less likely to be implanted (although see Scoboria, Mazzoni, Kirsch

  & Relyea, 2004 , for a discussion of plausibility).

  Hyman, Husband & Billings (1995) attempted to address the criticisms of

  Pezdek et al. (1997) by suggesting to their participants that they had experi-

  enced more unusual events. Following the methodology devised by Loftus &

  Pickrell (1995) they asked their participants to try to remember three events

  (two of which had occurred and one of which had not). Hyman et al. con-

  ducted two studies in which they suggested that their participants had expe-

  rienced one of the following false events when they were children:

  • an overnight hospitalization with a suspected ear infection (study 1);

  • a birthday party with a visit by a clown and pizza (study 1)

  • spilling a punch bowl at a wedding (study 2);

  • evacuating a grocery store when the sprinklers went off (study 2);

  • releasing the handbrake of a car in a car park and colliding with something

  (study 2).

  Their participants were interviewed three times over a three - week period

  and after each interview participants were asked to think about the events and

  192

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  try to remember more details before the next session. By the third and fi nal

  interview 89 – 95% of the ‘ real ’ events were recalled along with 25% of the ‘ false ’

  events (see also Hyman & Loftus, 1997 ). Thus, participants can be misled to

  report more unusual events from their childhood. However, critics argue that

  reporting that you remember spilling a punchbowl at a wedding does not

  compare to reporting that you remember being abused as a child – the latter

  is a much more negative, traumatic and emotionally - char
ged event. Nevertheless,

  one study has examined whether it is possible to mislead participants to report

  that they falsely remember negative, traumatic and emotionally - charged events

  from their childhood.

  Porter

  et al. (1999) , using a similar methodology to Loftus

  & Pickrell

  (1995) and Hyman

  et al. (1995) , suggested to participants that they had

  experienced serious negative events as children, such as:

  • a major medical procedure;

  • getting lost;

  • being seriously harmed by another child;

  • a serious animal attack.

  Porter et al. also verbally encouraged their participants to remember the

  events ( ‘ most people are able to retrieve lost memories if they try hard enough ’ ,

  Porter et al. , 1999 : 522), as well as asking them to think about this for fi ve

  minutes every night between interviews. By the third and fi nal interview,

  Porter et al. found that 54% of participants reported a ‘ full ’ or a ‘ partial ’ false

  memory. In the fi nal interview, Porter et al. (1999) also asked participants to

  deliberately fabricate an account of an event that did not occur. All three types

  of memory report (real, false and fabricated) were rated using a technique

  called the Memory Assessment Procedure (MAP) to investigate possible quali-

  tative differences between them. Porter et al. found that real and fabricated

  memories were rated as more vivid, more coherent and were given higher

  confi dence ratings than false memories. Fabricated memories were also rated

  as more stressful and contained more details than both real and false memories

  (Porter et al. , 1999 ; see also Pezdek & Talyor, 2000 ; Davies, 2001 ; Heaps &

  Nash, 2001 ; Loftus & Bernstein, 2005 ). Taken together, these studies indicate

  that, when misled by information provided by siblings or parents, participants

  will report that they remember unusual, negative, emotionally - charged and

  traumatic childhood events.

  In the studies described above, some kind of verbal instructions was used

  to encourage participants to try to remember the events (the only exception

  to this is Hyman et al. , 1995 , experiment 1). However, the extent to which,

  in the case of false reports of childhood events, the behaviour of the interviewer

  infl uenced the manner of recall is unclear. For example, Loftus & Pickrell

  ( 1995 : 722) noted that ‘ the interviewers maintained a pleasant and friendly

  manner whilst pressing for details ’ . In order to explore the possible role of

  social pressure in the development of false memories Ost, Foster, Costall &

  Recovered Memories

  193

  Bull (2005) followed a similar methodology to these previous studies, but

  trained their interviewers to interview in an appropriate and non - pressuring

  manner. Participants were asked about both positive and negative false events,

  similar to those used in previous studies, for example:

  • becoming lost;

  • a trip to the hospital;

  • a serious accident;

  • an eventful birthday party;

  • winning a contest.

  Levels of social pressure in the interviews were monitored by the participants

  (who were asked to rate, amongst other details, how pressured they felt to

  remember the event(s)) and by independent judges (who rated videotapes of

  the interviews). Overall levels of social pressure reported by both participants

  and independent judges were low. Despite the low levels of social pressure,

  Ost et al. (2005) found that seven out of 31 participants produced a ‘ full ’ or

  ‘ partial ’ report of a childhood event that did not occur. This study suggests

  that even minimal social pressure and repeated interviewing are suffi cient to

  lead some individuals to come to report events from their childhood that never

  occurred.

  Methodological l imitations of p arental m isinformation s tudies. There are

  several criticisms of the above studies that limit their generalizability to

  cases of delayed reports of childhood trauma. As already noted, Pezdek et al.

  (1997) argue that most of the false events that participants are asked to recall

  are events for which participants are likely to have a ‘ script ’ ; an event that is

  lower in plausibility and for which participants are less likely to have ‘ script -

  relevant ’ knowledge (such as childhood sexual abuse) is less likely to be falsely

  reported.

  Pezdek et al. (1997) tested this hypothesis by suggesting to Jewish and

  Catholic participants that they had taken part in both a Catholic ritual (receiv-

  ing Communion) and a Jewish ritual (Shabbot), neither of which had actually

  occurred. Pezdek et al. (1997) argue that the plausibility of having taken part

  in a Catholic ritual would be low for Jewish participants, that they would have

  less script - relevant knowledge to draw on, and vice versa. They found, in line

  with their predictions, that seven of the Catholic participants but none of the

  Jewish participants reported the false Catholic event and three Jewish partici-

  pants and one Catholic participant reported the Jewish false event. This shows

  that participants were more likely to remember the plausible false event than

  the implausible false event (i.e. Jewish participants were more likely to remem-

  ber the false Jewish ritual than the false Catholic ritual). In a second experi-

  ment, Pezdek et al. (1997) replicated the study by Loftus & Pickrell (1995)

  and extended it by suggesting to participants that they had (a) been lost in a

  shopping mall (a plausible event) and (b) that they had received an enema

  194

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  as a child (an implausible event). Pezdek et al. (1997) found that whilst three

  out of 20 participants falsely reported becoming lost in a shopping mall, none

  of the participants falsely reported receiving an enema. These experiments,

  therefore, suggest that the probability that participants can be misled to

  report false events from their childhood is likely to be a function of the plau-

  sibility of, or familiarity with, the event concerned (although see Scoboria

  et al. , 2004 , for a discussion of the differences between plausibility and script

  consistency).

  However, certain ‘ scripts ’ are more familiar in our culture than might at

  fi rst be imagined. Lynn & Pezzo ( 1994 ; cited in Lynn & Kirsch, 1996 ) found

  that participants were able to construct very convincing narratives of having

  been abducted by aliens even when given relatively little warning or further

  information. Lynn & Kirsch (1996) argue that narratives of alien abduction

  are so common in our culture that individuals are likely to have access to

  ‘ script - relevant ’ knowledge (see also Arndt & Greenberg, 1996 ). Given the

  large number of self - help books, media programmes, news articles and the like

  dealing with abuse, it is not inconceivable that some individuals could con-

  struct a false narrative of having been abused.

  A second, often cited limitation with parental misinformation studies is that

  the events participants are misled to report are not of an abusive or traumatic

&
nbsp; nature. However, Porter

  et al. (1999) showed that participants could be

  misled to report negatively charged and traumatic events, such as being victim

  to a serious animal attack. Although untestable in the laboratory, the possibil-

  ity remains that participants in situations other than a psychology experiment

  could be misled to report abusive events that did not occur (see also Hyman

  & Loftus,

  1997 ). In fact, evidence from retractors (individuals who have

  repudiated their earlier claims of abuse) suggests that this does occur (see de

  Rivera, 1998 ; Ost, Costall & Bull, 2001; 2002 ; Ost & Nunkoosing, in press ).

  Sometimes they became so convinced of the truth of their false beliefs that

  they initiated legal proceedings against their alleged abusers (de Rivera, 1998 ;

  Ost et al. , 2001; 2002 ). Worryingly, retractors reported that they experienced

  levels of social pressure and inappropriate questioning techniques not dissimi-

  lar to those that can lead to false confessions in police interrogations

  (Wrightsman & Kassin, 1993 ; Kassin, 1997 ; Ost, et al. , 2001 ; Gudjonsson,

  2003 ).

  A third limitation with parental misinformation studies is that it is not clear

  what degree of social pressure is required in order to lead participants to make

  false claims about the past (Ost et al. , 2005 ). For example, as mentioned above,

  Loftus & Pickrell ( 1995 : 722) note that their interviewer ‘ maintained a pleas-

  ant and friendly manner, whilst pressing for details ’ yet do not provide details

  of how participants were ‘ pressed for details ’ . Porter et al. (1999) also state

  that they employed a degree of verbal encouragement (

  ‘ most people can

  remember details if they try really hard ’ , p. 522). Malinoski & Lynn (1999)

  found that positive verbal encouragement led participants to report earlier (and

  more implausible) memories. Ost et al. (2005) found that, even when social

  Recovered Memories

  195

  pressure was kept to a minimum (by appropriately training interviewers not to

  pressure participants), a number of participants still reported false events from

  their childhood (see also Erdmann, Volbert & B ö hm, 2004 , for similar fi nd-

  ings with child witnesses). The role of social pressure in the genesis and devel-

 

‹ Prev