Book Read Free

Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Page 38

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  opment of false reports of childhood events is an important avenue for future

  research (Ost et al. 2001; 2005 ).

  A fi nal limitation of the parental misinformation studies is that they rely on

  parents or siblings to verify that certain events did, or did not, occur to par-

  ticipants when they were children. This may be unwise. Whilst it is probable

  that parents will be able to remember events in the lives of their young children

  with more confi dence than the children themselves (see Ost et al. , 2005 ), this

  in itself is no guarantee that they will remember them with great accuracy

  (Conte, 1999 ). Indeed, in Ost et al. (2005) , a few participants stated outright

  that their parents must have misremembered events or confused the participant

  with another sibling. Indeed, there is literature to suggest that parents are not

  the best at remembering events from their children

  ’ s past (Wenar,

  1961 ;

  Wenar & Coulter, 1962 ; see also Halverson, 1988 ). The problem, then, is

  that there is no way of knowing the ‘ ground truth ’ (i.e., whether an event did,

  or did not, occur) with any great certainty (Goff & Roediger, 1998 ).

  As a result, psychologists have developed novel methodologies that do

  not rely on obtaining information from outside sources. Garry, Manning,

  Loftus & Sherman (1996) devised one such methodology. They asked parti-

  cipants to complete a Life Events Inventory (LEI) which asked them to

  indicate the likelihood that a list of 40 events (e.g. ‘ got in trouble for calling

  911 ’ ; ‘ broke a window with your hand ’ ) had happened to them before the

  age of ten. Participants were asked to provide a rating for each event on a

  scale from 1 ( ‘ defi nitely did not happen ’ ) to 8 ( ‘ defi nitely did happen ’ ). Two

  weeks later participants were asked to imagine some of the events (including

  some that had been given a rating of ‘ defi nitely did not happen ’ ) and answer

  questions about them. These events were referred to as

  ‘ critical items

  ’ .

  Participants were then asked to complete the LEI again (on the pretext that

  the original had been lost). Garry

  et al. (1996) found that the

  likelihood

  ratings were more likely to change for critical items compared to those that

  had not been imagined. Garry et al. (1996) concluded that the mere act of

  imagining a false event increased participants ’ subjective confi dence that it had

  occurred.

  This is known as the imagination infl ation effect and it appears to be robust.

  It occurs when participants are asked to imagine recent events (Goff

  &

  Roediger, 1998 ) as well as implausible and bizarre events (Mazzoni, Loftus &

  Kirsch, 2001 ; Thomas & Loftus, 2002 ; Pezdek, Blandon - Gitlin & Gabbay,

  2006 ). For example, in a recent experiment, participants were asked to perform,

  or imagine performing, familiar ( ‘ check the Pepsi machine for change ’ ) or

  bizarre ( ‘ propose marriage to the Pepsi machine ’ ) actions during a campus

  walk. Two weeks later, some participants misremembered performing both

  familiar and unfamiliar actions that they had, in fact, only imagined themselves

  196

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  doing (Seamon, Philbin & Harrison, 2006 ). However, critics question whether

  the changes in likelihood ratings are because participants have developed

  genuine false beliefs or whether they are the result of regression towards the

  mean. This is a statistical artefact where extreme scores on a measure (i.e.,

  ‘ defi nitely did not happen ’ ) are likely, by chance alone, to become less extreme

  (i.e., ‘ possibly might have happened ’ ) when that measure is taken a second

  time (Pezdek & Eddy, 2001 ; see reply by Garry, Sharman, Wade, Hunt &

  Smith, 2001 ).

  A second innovative paradigm is called the false feedback method (Bernstein,

  Laney, Morris & Loftus, 2005 ; Laney, Morris, Bernstein, Wakefi eld & Loftus,

  2008 ). In false feedback studies, participants are asked to complete a question-

  naire about their food preferences. This questionnaire is then fed into a com-

  puter for analysis, but the results of the analysis are, in fact, bogus. When the

  results of the questionnaire are interpreted, the experimenter tells that partici-

  pant that the computer analysis indicates that, as a child, the participant got

  sick after eating too much of a certain food (either eggs or dill pickles).

  Bernstein and colleagues found that participants who were given this false

  feedback were more confi dent that this event had indeed happened than

  control participants (Bernstein et al. , 2005 ).

  Ongoing c hallenges in f alse m emory r esearch

  Despite all the advances made in recent years there is still a number of linger-

  ing issues that need to be resolved. The fi rst is that it diffi cult to be sure

  whether we are really implanting false memories in laboratory studies. The dif-

  fi culty is that any number of intervening processes could produce the same

  output (in this case a claim to ‘ remember ’ ). It could be due to a report bias

  – a tendency to say ‘ yes ’ . It could be due to increases in subjective confi dence

  – being more confi dent that an event could have occurred. It could be due to

  holding a genuine belief that the event occurred in the absence of a clear

  memory of the event (Ost, 2003 ) or it could be due to having developed a

  clear, yet inaccurate, memory of the alleged event(s). There is certainly evi-

  dence that some individuals will embellish suggested false beliefs with details

  from their own autobiography (see Bernstein et al. , 2005 ). There is also evi-

  dence that participants develop fairly detailed false recollections of events that

  are suggested to them (Bernstein et al. , 2005 ; Ost, Granhag, Udell & Roos

  af Hjelmst ä ter, 2008 ). One current challenge, then, is to investigate which of

  these processes are responsible for a given output and the manner in which

  these processes are related – in other words, how a belief becomes a fully -

  fl edged memory (Mazzoni

  & Kirsch,

  2002 ; Scoboria, Mazzoni, Kirsch

  &

  Relyea, 2004 ; Ost et al. , 2008 ).

  The second, related, concern centres on the likely consequence of false

  beliefs or memories (Smeets et al. , 2005 ). Whilst there is evidence that recov-

  ering abuse memories has serious and negative impacts on people

  ’ s lives

  Recovered Memories

  197

  (Loftus, 1997a ), it is diffi cult to establish whether participants in psychological

  experiments continue to accept the truth of the implanted beliefs or memories

  once they have left the laboratory. As Smeets and colleagues point out, one

  measure of how much someone has truly accepted a suggestion is the extent

  to which they would be prepared to change their behaviour as a result of that

  suggestion. Recently, psychologists have begun to tackle this question. In

  Bernstein et al. ’ s (2005) false feedback study, participants were give a question-

  naire about an imaginary barbecue which asked them to indicate which kinds

  of food they wo
uld be likely to eat. They found participants who believed the

  false feedback about becoming sick as a child after eating too many eggs or

  pickles indicated that they would be less likely to choose to eat those foods

  than those who did not believe the feedback. Thus, this experiment demon-

  strates that false feedback about the likelihood of past events can infl uence

  later behaviour but, importantly, only for those participants who believe the

  feedback. In a novel twist, Laney, Morris, Bernstein, Wakefi eld & Loftus

  (2008) found that the same kind of false feedback could be used to positive

  effect to convince participants that, as a child, they really enjoyed eating a

  healthy food – in this case, asparagus.

  Finally, there are concerns over the defi nition of the subject matter.

  The research on false memory has grown almost exponentially over the

  past few years. Pezdek & Lam (2007) analysed the psychological literature

  and demonstrated that, although the number of studies referring to

  ‘ false memory

  ’ increased, the proportion of them dealing with memories

  of entire events (e.g., ‘ rich ’ false memories) has remained low (13%). The

  highest proportion of studies referred to as ‘ false memory ’ in fact deals with

  the DRM word list paradigm (42%) (see also DePrince, Allard, Oh & Freyd,

  2004 ). Pezdek & Lam (2007) questioned whether these DRM experiments

  are really studies of ‘ false memory ’ in any meaningful sense or whether they

  should be referred to as

  ‘ memory fl aws ’ (see Wade

  et al. ,

  2007 , for a

  rebuttal).

  Conclusion

  The psychological research presented in this chapter has shown that, under

  certain circumstances outside of therapy (e.g. repeated suggestive interviewing

  in a psychological laboratory), people will come to report that they remember

  events that did not occur. Further research is needed to examine what it is

  about these people, or the circumstances in which they fi nd themselves, that

  leads them to make false memory reports, which can be vivid and compelling,

  yet entirely inaccurate. Practitioners and policy - makers therefore need to be

  sensitive to these issues if we are to deal appropriately with cases like Alice ’ s,

  to prevent future miscarriages of justice and to ensure that genuine victims of

  abuse receive the support they need.

  198

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Acknowledgements

  My thanks to Kimberley Wade for constructive comments on a draft of this

  chapter, part of which was published as Ost, J. & Wade, K. (2007) . Can we

  forget bad memories? British False Memory Society Newsletter , 15 , 4 – 8.

  References

  Anderson , M. C. & Green , C. ( 2001 ). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive

  control . Nature , 410 , 366 – 369 .

  Anderson , M. C. , Ochsner , K. N. , Kuhl , B. , Cooper , J. , Robertson , E. , Gabrieli , S.

  W. , Glover , G. H. & Gabrieli , J. D. E. ( 2004 ). Neural systems underlying the

  suppression of unwanted memories . Science , 303 , 232 – 235 .

  Arndt , J. & Greenberg , J. ( 1996 ). Fantastic accounts can take many forms: False

  memory construction? Yes. Escape from self? We don ’ t think so . Psychological

  Inquiry , 7 , 127 – 132 .

  Bekerian , D. A. & Bowers , J. M. ( 1983 ). Eyewitness testimony: Were we misled? Journal

  of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 9 , 139 – 145 .

  Bernstein , D. M. , Laney , C. , Morris , E. K. & Loftus , E. F. ( 2005 ). False memories

  about food can lead to food avoidance . Social Cognition , 23 , 11 – 34 .

  Blume , E. ( 1990 ). Secret survivors: Uncovering incest and its after - effects in women .

  New York : John Wiley & Sons .

  Brainerd , C. J. & Reyna , V. F. ( 1998 ). When things that were never experienced are

  easier to ‘ remember ’ than things that were . Psychological Science , 9 , 484 – 489 .

  Brand , N. (Ed.) ( 2007 ). Fractured families: The untold anguish of the falsely accused .

  Bradford - on - Avon : BFMS .

  Brandon , S. , Boakes , J. , Glaser , D. , Green , R. , MacKeith , J. & Whewell , P. ( 1997 ).

  Reported recovered memories of child sexual abuse: Recommendations for good

  practice and implications for training, continuing professional development and

  research . Psychiatric Bulletin , 21 , 663 – 665 .

  Brown , D. , Schefl in , A. W. & Hammond , D. C. ( 1998 ). Memory, trauma treatment,

  and the law . New York : W. W. Norton .

  Brown , R. , Goldstein , E. & Bjorklund , D. F. ( 2000 ). The history and zeitgeist of the

  repressed - false - memory debate: Scientifi c and sociological perspectives on sug-

  gestibility . In D. F. Bjorklund (Ed.), False memory creation in children and adults

  (pp. 1 – 30 ). Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum .

  Bulevich , J. B. , Roediger , H. L. , Balota , D. A. & Butler , A. C. ( 2006 ). Failures to

  fi nd suppression of episodic memories in the think/no - think paradigm . Memory

  & Cognition , 34 , 1569 – 1577 .

  Cameron ,

  C. (

  1996 ).

  Comparing amnesic and nonamnesic survivors of childhood

  sexual abuse: A longitudinal study . In K. Pezdek & W. P. Banks (Eds.), The

  recovered memory/false memory debate (pp. 41 – 68 ). San Diego : Academic Press .

  Catchpole , Z. ( 2003 ). Doctor led his patient to make sex claims . The Daily Mail ,

  9 September.

  Conte , J. R. ( 1999 ). Memory, research, and the law: Future directions . In L. M.

  Williams & V. L. Banyard (Eds.), Trauma and memory (pp. 77 – 92 ). Thousand

  Oaks, CA : Sage Publications .

  Recovered Memories

  199

  Conway ,

  M. A.

  (

  1997 ).

  Recovered memories and false memories .

  Oxford :

  Oxford

  University Press .

  Crombag , H. F. M. , Wagenaar , W. A. & van Koppen , P. J. ( 1996 ). Crashing memories

  and the problem of

  ‘ source monitoring

  ’ .

  Applied Cognitive Psychology ,

  10 ,

  95 – 104 .

  Davies , G. M. ( 2001 ). Is it possible to discriminate between true and false memories?

  In G. M. Davies & T. Dalgleish (Eds.), Recovered memories: Seeking the middle

  ground (pp. 153 – 174 ). Chichester : John Wiley & Sons .

  de Rivera , J. ( 1998 ). Relinquishing believed - in imaginings: Narratives of people who

  have repudiated false accusations . In J. de Rivera & T. R. Sarbin (Eds.), Believed -

  in imaginings: The narrative reconstruction of reality (pp. 169 – 188 ). Washington,

  DC : American Psychological Association .

  Deese , J. ( 1959 ). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in

  immediate recall . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 58 , 17 – 22 .

  DePrince , A. P. , Allard , C. B. , Oh , H. & Freyd , J. J. ( 2004 ). What ’ s in a name for

  memory errors? Implications and ethical issues arising from the use of the term

  ‘ false memory

  ’ for errors in memory for details

  .

  Ethics

  & Behavior ,

  14 ,

  201 – 233 .

  Depue , B.
E. , Banich , M. T. & Curran , T. ( 2006 ). Suppression of emotional and

  nonemotional content in memory. Effects of repetition on cognitive control

  .

  Psychological Science , 17 , 441 – 447 .

  Depue B. E

  ,

  Curran T. & Banich M. T.

  (

  2007 ).

  Prefrontal regions orchestrate

  suppression of emotional memories via a two - phase process . Science , 317 , 215 –

  219 .

  Erdelyi , M. H. ( 2006 ). The unifi ed theory of repression . Behavioral and Brain Sciences ,

  29 , 499 – 551 .

  Erdmann , K. , Volbert , R. & B ö hm , C. ( 2004 ). Children report suggested events even

  when interviewed in a non - suggestive manner: what are its implications for cred-

  ibility assessment? Applied Cognitive Psychology , 18 , 589 – 611 .

  Freyd ,

  J. J.

  (

  1998 ).

  Science in the memory debate

  .

  Ethics

  & Behavior ,

  8 ,

  101 –

  113 .

  Freyd , J. J. & Gleaves , D. H. ( 1996 ). ‘ Remembering ’ words not presented in lists:

  Relevance to the current recovered/false memory controversy

  .

  Journal of

  Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 22 , 811 – 813 .

  Gallo , D. A. , Roberts , M. J. & Seamon , J. G. ( 1997 ). Remembering words not pre-

  sented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories? Psychonomic Bulletin and

  Review , 4 , 271 – 276 .

  Garry , M. & Loftus , E. G. ( 2004 ). I am Freud ’ s brain . Skeptical Inquirer , May/June,

  16 – 18 .

  Garry , M. , Manning , C. G. , Loftus , E. F. & Sherman , S. J. ( 1996 ). Imagination infl a-

  tion: Imagining a childhood event infl ates confi dence that it occurred . Psychological

  Bulletin and Review , 3 , 208 – 214 .

  Garry , M. , Sharman , S. J. , Wade. K. A. , Hunt , M. J. & Smith , P. J. ( 2001 ). Imagination

  infl ation is a fact, not an artifact . Memory and Cognition , 29 , 719 – 729 .

  Goff ,

  L. M.

  & Roediger ,

  H. L.

  (

  1998 ).

  Imagination infl ation for action events:

  Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections . Memory and Cognition , 26 ,

  20 – 33 .

  Goodman , G. S. , Ghetti , S. , Quas , J. A. , Edelstein , R. S. , Alexander , K. W. , Redlich ,

  A. D. , Cordon , I. M. & Jones , D. P. H. ( 2003 ). A prospective study of memory

 

‹ Prev