Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Home > Other > Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions > Page 59
Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions Page 59

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  ological issue will undoubtedly yield results of relatively limited practical utility.

  Accordingly, in reviewing the existing research, individuals are urged to do so

  cautiously and critically.

  One potential solution to these problems is to employ a different method

  from that traditionally used in research on evaluating truthfulness: a series of

  case studies in which verbal and nonverbal behaviour are examined and deter-

  minations of truthfulness are made on an individual basis via an empirically -

  grounded and experience

  - informed approach (see below). With such an

  approach, quantitative and qualitative statistics could be utilized. While indi-

  vidual cases should be evaluated qualitatively, individual cases can thereafter

  be aggregated and analysed quantitatively. Not only would this approach serve

  to overcome the limitations discussed above, it would also help focus research-

  ers on developing better - informed approaches to evaluating truthfulness as

  opposed to searching for the all - elusive ‘ signs ’ of deception. As expanded on

  below, such diagnostic signs have yet to reveal themselves and, moreover, are

  likely not to exist. Of course, single - case research designs come with their own

  complexities. That is, they are labour - intensive and costly, which may explain

  why this approach has never gained favour in such a competitive, publication -

  driven arena. Nevertheless, we argue that case studies will prove very useful in

  understanding how to evaluate truthfulness in applied contexts.

  308

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Pre - training accuracy in evaluating truthfulness

  One of the major fi ndings in the research on evaluating truthfulness is that

  it has been repeatedly demonstrated that most individuals, irrespective of

  professional background, are poor at distinguishing truths from lies. Ekman

  & O ’ Sullivan (1991) examined the ability of a large group of professionals and

  non - professionals, including police offi cers, secret service agents, polygraphers,

  psychiatrists and college students, to evaluate truthfulness by showing them a

  series of videos of individuals lying or telling the truth. Some video clips

  depicted individuals lying or telling the truth about their opinions on sensitive

  subjects, such as the death penalty, while others depicted individuals lying or

  telling the truth about their participation or non - participation in a mock crime.

  The researchers showed that there was no relationship between gender and

  the ability of the participants to tell who was lying and who was telling the

  truth. There was no relationship between years as an investigator/professional

  and the ability to evaluate truthfulness. There was also no relationship between

  confi dence in one ’ s ability to evaluate truthfulness and one ’ s actual ability.

  Men have been found to be more confi dent in their wrong decisions (e.g.,

  Porter, Woodworth & Birt, 2000 ), once again highlighting the importance

  of considering individual differences. The major fi nding from Ekman

  &

  O ’ Sullivan ’ s ( 1991 ) study was that, as a group, participants were shown to be

  able to differentiate truth from lies only at chance levels. Only one subgroup,

  the secret service agents, was demonstrated to evaluate truthfulness at a level

  higher than chance (64%), although only marginally so and not to levels neces-

  sary for effective job performance. The fl avour of Ekman & O ’ Sullivan ’ s results

  has been replicated with different stimuli and participants, suggesting that most

  people, irrespective of profession and experience, cannot accurately evaluate

  truthfulness (Porter et al ., 2000 ).

  Roadblocks to the accurate evaluation of truthfulness

  Research has demonstrated that there are a number of roadblocks that prevent

  individuals from accurately evaluating truthfulness (Ekman,

  1992 ; Herv

  é ,

  Cooper & Yuille, 2008 ; Vrij, 2000 ). Heading the list is a lack of evidence -

  based knowledge and skills specifi c to evaluating truthfulness, which results in

  individuals relying on their ‘ experience ’ and/or popular myths (see below).

  More generally, another roadblock refl ects a lack of critical thought. Critical

  thinking is a necessary, but not suffi cient, component in conducting evalua-

  tions and to evaluating truthfulness within such evaluations. Each roadblock

  is discussed in turn.

  In terms of lack of knowledge, research indicates that most individuals do

  not know what lies and truths look like (Akehurst, Kohnken, Vrij & Bull,

  1996 ; Ekman & O ’ Sullivan, 1991 ; Porter et al ., 2000 ; Vrij, 2004 ). It is clear

  Evaluating Truthfulness

  309

  that people rely on certain clues related to what they think lies and truths look

  like; however, research indicates that, more often than not, such heavily relied

  upon clues (e.g., all liars will experience anxiety/fear and, therefore, avoid eye

  contact; Ekman, 1992) are wrong. Such clues are simply myths, often perpetu-

  ated in the media and in professional manuals, but lacking empirical support.

  With regards to skills, if the skills required for the job are lacking in breadth

  and depth, the job cannot be performed adequately. For instance, if evidence -

  based approaches are not used for the assessment of risk for recidivism, there

  will be substantial false

  - positive and false

  - negative errors made (Monahan,

  1981 ). The same is true with respect to evaluating truthfulness: if the right

  ‘ tools for the job ’ are absent, it is impossible to do that job. This is especially

  notable in this context given that the vast individual differences in how people

  reveal their lies dictates a need for a vast arsenal for detecting lies. Nevertheless,

  it is sometimes the case that, even if people have the right tools for the job,

  they are using them in the wrong way. For example, individuals could be

  trained in proven approaches for investigative interviewing and in evaluating

  verbal clues to credibility (i.e., two approaches integral to evaluating truthful-

  ness), but such skills could still be poorly applied (i.e., rigidly rather than fl uidly

  and fl exibly). It is likely that this especially occurs over time; that is, too often

  individuals fall prey to drift, thus illustrating the need for practice and quality

  control. Finally, sometimes individuals fail to use the tools at all. The conse-

  quences of the fi rst generation of risk assessments studies are a case in point.

  In this generation, clinicians relied on their clinical opinion as opposed to

  empirically validated risk inventories, and errors were made more often that

  not (Steadman & Cocozza, 1974 ; Thornberry & Jacoby, 1979 ; for a review,

  see Monahan et al ., 2001 ). A similar lesson has been learned in the area of

  evaluating truthfulness: empirically validated tools are needed for the job!

  Another roadblock relates to failing to consider how knowledge and skills

  change over time. Within any area in psychology − and most other disciplines

  for that matter − knowledge and skills change, as the evidence to support them

  changes. Consistent with most asses
sment practices, the accurate evaluation

  of truthfulness requires individuals to stay up to date with the literature.

  Moreover, professionals have an ethical obligation to stay current in the litera-

  ture related to their areas of practice. Keeping up to date with the literature

  and implementing suggestions into clinical practice will prevent drift and

  related problems.

  Although proper knowledge and skills are clearly important, a lack of criti-

  cal thought is arguably the major roadblock to accurately evaluating truthful-

  ness. Unfortunately, it is not uncommon for individuals to fail to evaluate

  each case on its own merits and to adopt a ‘ cookie cutter ’ approach to the

  task at hand. Such lack of objectivity can frequently be traced to internal or

  external factors. In terms of the former, poor psychological and/or physical

  health and/or egos too often impact on evaluators ’ decision - making. With

  regard to external factors, individuals may be pressed for time because of an

  onerous workload or unreasonable deadlines. Moreover, lack of objectivity

  310

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  may relate to being biased a priori against the person being assessed. Lack of

  critical thinking also leads to a failure to consider alternative hypotheses. Just

  because a given question appears to be a ‘ no - brainer ’ does not mean that it

  should be treated as such. Indeed, the decisions that are made in the forensic

  arena affect the lives and well - being of many individuals and, therefore, alter-

  native hypotheses must be considered before a conclusion is made. Finally,

  lack of critical thinking may lead to a failure to check and double - check con-

  clusions drawn. The approach to evaluating truthfulness that is introduced in

  this chapter requires individuals to frequently re - evaluate their conclusions in

  light of the evidence that formed their conclusions. In fact, the business

  of evaluating truthfulness is so complex that it requires a conscientious, quasi -

  perfectionist approach.

  The bottom line is that roadblocks to evaluating truthfulness need to be

  overcome. That is, individuals need to know about evidence - based practice in

  evaluating truthfulness. To this end, the following section outlines empirically -

  based training components for the accurate evaluation of truthfulness. These

  training components form the basis of the approach introduced in the follow-

  ing section.

  Evidenced - based training components for

  the evaluation of truthfulness

  A review of research on clinical decision - making in general and evaluating

  truthfulness in particular suggests that training in evaluating truthfulness

  involves four major areas: (i) bad habits need to be unlearned; (ii) evidence -

  based knowledge about evaluating truthfulness needs to be acquired; (iii)

  empirically - validated tools need to be learned and practiced; and (iv) a method

  that emphasizes critical thinking in evaluating truthfulness needs to be used;

  the latter of which is perhaps the most diffi cult area to train. Each component

  is discussed in turn below.

  Unlearning bad habits

  Unlearning bad habits requires knowledge. Without basic, empirically - based

  knowledge about evaluating truthfulness, individuals tend to make common

  errors. As some researchers have suggested that the state of the research in

  evaluating truthfulness is not yet adequate to support its use in practice (e.g.,

  Vrij, Mann & Fisher, 2006 ), it is argued that, at the very least, individuals

  should be informed of the errors, or myths, that riddle their work, as well as

  methods to avoid committing such errors. Although many myths exist (see

  Ekman, 1992 ; Vrij, 2000 ), they can be broadly categorized as being either

  experiential or societal in nature, although these are not necessarily mutually

  exclusive categories.

  Evaluating Truthfulness

  311

  Experientially - driven myths stem from individuals

  ’ personal experiences.

  For example, some people rely on what has been termed the ‘ me ’ theory of

  behavioural assessment (Ekman,

  1992 ). That is, they assume people will

  behave as they do when telling the truth or lying. For example, when using

  the ‘ me ’ theory, if someone avoids eye contact when lying, this person will

  view others as lying when they avert their gaze. Unfortunately, this approach

  more often than not results in what has been termed the

  ‘ idiosyncratic

  error ’ − not taking into account the various unique behaviours of individuals

  ( ibid .). Not only may individuals differ within a culture (e.g., some people

  often rub their noses; others manipulate the hair on their face routinely),

  research has begun to identify important cross - cultural differences as well (e.g.,

  eye gaze has been found to vary across cultures; McCarthy, Lee, Itakura &

  Muir, 2006 ).

  Some individuals, particularly those with experience in evaluating truthful-

  ness, often rely on ‘ gut instincts ’ or on ‘ intuitions ’ about whether or not

  someone is telling the truth or lying. It is not suggested that individuals should

  ignore their instincts or intuitions; indeed, a recent review of research on

  intuition has demonstrated that, at least occasionally, intuition can point

  people in the right direction (Hodgkinson, Langan

  - Fox

  & Sadler

  - Smith,

  2008 ). However, we suggest that instincts/intuitions should not be viewed as

  answers in and of themselves. Rather, they should be viewed as hypotheses to

  be tested against the available evidence. If the data do not support the person ’ s

  intuition/instinct, there should be no reason for a conclusion to be made

  simply on intuition/instinct.

  Another experientially - driven myth concerns the relationship between expe-

  rience and accuracy in evaluating truthfulness. Regarding the fi ndings on

  experience, the research has been mixed. Some (e.g., Ekman & O ’ Sullivan,

  1991 ) report no benefi t from experience, but others (e.g., Mann, Vrij & Bull,

  2004 ) have shown a positive benefi t from experience on detection of lies.

  Experience can also produce overconfi dence, which unfortunately too often

  leads evaluators to become myopic and, therefore, to seek the same false clues

  time and time again. The research is clear: if people rely solely on their own

  idiosyncrasies and/or experiences as the basis for their judgements for evaluat-

  ing truthfulness, they are likely to be wrong most of the time (see Ekman,

  1992 ; The Global Deception Team, 2006 ).

  Societal - driven myths refl ect shared beliefs about

  ‘ the sign or signs

  ’ of

  deception or of truth - telling (Ekman, 1992 ; Ford, 2006 ). In terms of truth -

  telling, there are the common myths that maintaining eye contact and lack of

  observable anxiety are reliable signs of honesty. Conversely, there are the

  opposite myths that sweating, anxiety and/or fear are signs indicative of decep-

  tion. This type of myth unfortunately results in what Ekman (1992) has termed

  the ‘ Othello error ’ (after Shakespeare ’ s
tragedy, Othello ). Othello wrongfully

  believed that his wife, Desdemona, had been unfaithful to him. When he

  confronted her about her suspected infi delities, she presented as fearful.

  Desdemona had considerable reason to be fearful, as Othello had already

  312

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  murdered her suspected lover. Othello ’ s error occurred when he misattributed

  Desdemona ’ s fear of being disbelieved as evidence of her guilt. It is important

  to understand that fear of being disbelieved looks the same as fear of being

  caught in a lie. That is, spotting an emotion only informs us about its kind,

  not its source or cause (Ekman, 2003 ). Consequently, it is important to be

  mindful of the reasons why someone may be experiencing an emotion in a

  given circumstance.

  Proponents of neuro - linguistic programming (NLP) suggest that looking

  up and to the left is associated with lying. However, there is no research to

  support this proposition. Not only does the research indicate that the direction

  of the eye gaze has no meaning, averting eye gaze could be a clue to concen-

  tration, could refl ect one ’ s attempt not to be infl uenced by the facial expression

  of interviewees/peers, and/or could be associated with lying. Again, the

  research is clear: there is no Pinocchio response indicative of deception (Ekman,

  1992 ). That is, there is no particular physiological, physical or psychological

  response that individuals demonstrate when they lie that they do not also

  demonstrate when they are under stress and/or concentrating.

  An error that refl ects experiential infl uences but tends to be common within

  society, at least in Western culture, concerns the tendency to focus uncritically

  on verbal information to the detriment of nonverbal information, which

  appears to refl ect the overemphasis on language development. Indeed, while

  children are known to be relatively profi cient in nonverbal communication,

  adults – through socialization – have learned to focus more on the spoken

  word. As a result, facial expressions of emotions are, for example, usually

  ignored due to verbal overrides, particularly if the emotion displayed is at odds

  with what is being said. This speaks to the importance of active listening and

 

‹ Prev