Not Born Yesterday

Home > Other > Not Born Yesterday > Page 2
Not Born Yesterday Page 2

by Hugo Mercier


  truth. In the face of such motivations, open vigilance mecha-

  nisms come to be used, perversely, to identify not the most

  plausible but the most implausible views.

  From the most intuitive to the most preposterous, if we want

  to understand why some mistaken views catch on, we must un-

  derstand how open vigilance works.

  Uptake

  At the end of the book, you should have a grasp on how you de-

  cide what to believe and who to trust. You should know more

  about how miserably unsuccessful most attempts at mass persua-

  sion are, from the most banal— advertising, proselytizing—to

  the most extreme— brainwashing, subliminal influence. You

  should have some clues about why (some) mistaken ideas man-

  in t r o d u c t i o n xix

  age to spread, while (some) valuable insights prove so difficult

  to diffuse. You should understand why I once gave a fake doctor

  twenty euros.

  I do hope you come to accept the core of the book’s argument.

  But, please, don’t just take my word for it. I’d hate to be proven

  wrong by my own readers.

  1

  THE CASE FOR GULLIBILITY

  for millennia, people have accepted many bizarre beliefs

  and have been persuaded to engage in irrational be hav iors (or

  so it appears). These beliefs and be hav iors gave credence to the

  idea that the masses are gullible. In real ity I believe the story is

  more complicated (or even completely diff er ent, as we’ll see in

  the following chapters). But I must start by laying out the case

  for gullibility.

  In 425 BCE, Athens had been locked for years in a mutually

  destructive war with Sparta. At the Battle of Pylos, the Athenian

  naval and ground forces managed to trap Spartan troops on the

  island of Sphacteria. Seeing that a significant number of their elite

  were among the captives, the Spartan leaders sued for peace, of-

  fering advantageous terms to Athens. The Athenians declined

  the offer. The war went on, Sparta regained the edge, and when

  a (temporary) peace treaty was signed, in 421 BCE, the terms

  were much less favorable to Athens. This blunder was only one

  of a series of terrible Athenian decisions. Some were morally

  repellent— kil ing all the citizens of a conquered city— others

  were strategically disastrous— launching a doomed expedition

  to Sicily. In the end, Athens lost the war and would never regain

  its former power.

  1

  2 ch ap t er 1

  In 1212, a “multitude of paupers” in France and Germany took

  the cross to fight the infidels and reclaim Jerusalem for the Catho-

  lic Church.1 As many of these paupers were very young, this

  movement was dubbed the Children’s Crusade. The youth made

  it to Saint- Denis, prayed in the cathedral, met the French king,

  hoped for a miracle. No miracle happened. What can be expected

  of an army of untrained, unfunded, disor ga nized preteens? Not

  much, which is what they achieved: none reached Jerusalem, and

  many died along the way.

  In the mid- eighteenth century the Xhosa, a pastoralist people

  of South Africa, were suffering under the newly imposed British

  rule. Some of the Xhosa believed kil ing all their cattle and burn-

  ing their crops would raise a ghost army that would fend off the

  British. They sacrificed thousands of heads of cattle and set

  fire to their fields. No ghost army arose. The British stayed. The

  Xhosa died.

  On December 4, 2016, Edgar Maddison Welch entered the

  Comet Ping Pong pizzeria in Washington, DC, carry ing an as-

  sault rifle, a revolver, and a shotgun. He wasn’t there to rob the

  restaurant. Instead, he wanted to make sure that no children were

  being held hostage in the basement. There had been rumors that

  the Clintons— the former U.S. president and his wife, then cam-

  paigning for the presidency— were running a sex trafficking

  ring, and that Comet Ping Pong was one of their lairs. Welch was

  arrested and is now serving a prison sentence.

  Blind Trust

  Scholars, feeling superior to the masses, have often explained

  these questionable decisions and weird beliefs by a human dis-

  position to be overly trusting, a disposition that would make the

  masses instinctively defer to charismatic leaders regardless of

  t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 3

  their competence or motivations, believe what ever they hear or

  read irrespective of its plausibility, and follow the crowd even

  when doing so leads to disaster. This explanation— the masses

  are credulous— has proven very influential throughout history

  even if, as will soon become clear, it is misguided.

  Why did the Athenians lose the war against Sparta? Starting

  with Thucydides, chronicler of the Peloponnesian War, many

  commentators have blamed the influence of demagogues such

  as Cleon, a parvenu “very power ful with the multitude,” who was

  deemed responsible for some of the war’s worst blunders.2 A

  generation later, Plato extended Thucydides’s argument into

  a general indictment of democracy. For Plato, the rule of the

  many unavoidably gives rise to leaders who, “having a mob

  entirely at [their] disposal,” turn into tyrants.3

  Why would a bunch of youngsters abandon their homes in the

  vain hope of invading a faraway land? They were responding to

  the calls for a new crusade launched by Pope Innocent III, their

  supposed credulity inspiring the legend of the Pied Piper of

  Hamelin, whose magic flute grants him absolute power over all

  the children who hear it.4 People’s crusades also help explain the

  accusations that emerged in the Enlightenment, by the likes of

  the Baron d’Holbach, who chastised the Christian Church for

  “deliver[ing] mankind into [the] hands of [despots and tyrants]

  as a herd of slaves, of whom they may dispose at their

  plea sure.”5

  Why did the Xhosa kill their cattle? A century earlier, the Mar-

  quis de Condorcet, a central figure of the French Enlighten-

  ment, suggested that members of small- scale socie ties suffered

  from the “credulity of the first dupes,” putting too much faith in

  “charlatans and sorcerers.”6 The Xhosa seem to fit this picture.

  They were taken in by Nongqawuse, a young prophetess who

  had had visions of the dead rising to fight the British, and of a

  4 ch ap t er 1

  new world in which “nobody would ever lead a troubled life.

  People would get what ever they wanted. Every thing would be

  available in abundance.”7 Who would say no to that? Apparently

  not the Xhosa.

  Why did Edgar Maddison Welch risk jail to deliver non ex-

  is tent children from the non ex is tent basement of a harmless

  pizzeria? He had been listening to Alex Jones, the charismatic

  radio host who specializes in the craziest conspiracy theories,

  from the great Satanist takeover of Amer ica to government-

  sponsored calamities.8 For a time, Jones took up the idea that

  the Clintons and their aides led an organ ization trafficki
ng

  children for sex. As a Washington Post reporter put it, Jones and

  his ilk can peddle their wild theories because “gullibility helps

  create a market for it.”9

  All of these observers agree that people are often credulous,

  easily accept unsubstantiated arguments, and are routinely talked

  into stupid and costly be hav iors. Indeed, it is difficult to find an

  idea that so well unites radically diff er ent thinkers. Preachers lam-

  baste the “credulous multitude” who believe in gods other than

  the preachers’ own.10 Atheists point out “the almost superhuman

  gullibility” of those who follow religious preachers, what ever

  their god might be.11 Conspiracy theorists feel superior to the

  “mind controlled sheeple” who accept the official news.12 De-

  bunkers think conspiracy theorists “super gul ible” for believing

  the tall tales peddled by angry entertainers.13 Conservative writ-

  ers accuse the masses of criminal credulity when they revolt,

  prodded by shameless demagogues and driven mad by conta-

  gious emotions. Old- school leftists explain the passivity of the

  masses by their ac cep tance of the dominant ideology: “The in-

  dividual lives his repression ‘freely’ as his own life: he desires

  what he is supposed to desire,” instead of acting on “his original

  instinctual needs.”14

  t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 5

  For most of history, the concept of widespread credulity has

  been fundamental to our understanding of society. The assump-

  tion that people are easily taken in by demagogues runs across

  Western thought, from ancient Greece to the Enlightenment,

  creating “po liti cal philosophy’s central reason for skepticism

  about democracy.”15 Con temporary commenters still deplore

  how easily politicians sway voters by “pander[ing] to their gull-

  ibility.”16 But the ease with which people can be influenced has

  never been so (apparently) well illustrated as through a number

  of famous experiments conducted by social psychologists since

  the 1950s.

  Psychologists of Gullibility

  First came Solomon Asch. In his most famous experiment he

  asked people to answer a simple question: Which of three lines

  (depicted in figure 1) is as long as the first line?17 The three

  lines were clearly of diff er ent lengths, and one of them was an

  obvious match for the first. Yet participants made a mistake

  more than 30 percent of the time. Why would people provide

  such blatantly wrong answers? Before each participant was

  asked for their opinion, several participants had already re-

  plied. Unbeknownst to the actual participant, these other par-

  ticipants were confederates, planted by the experimenter. On

  some trials, all the confederates agreed on one of the wrong

  answers. These confederates held no power over the partici-

  pants, who did not even know them, and they were providing

  plainly wrong answers. Stil , more than 60 percent of partici-

  pants chose at least once to fol ow the group’s lead. A textbook

  written by Serge Moscovici, an influential social psychologist,

  describes these results as “one of the most dramatic illustra-

  tions of conformity, of blindly going along with the group, even

  6 ch ap t er 1

  A

  B

  C

  Figure 1. The lines in the Asch conformity experiments. Source: Wikipedia.

  when the individual realizes that by doing so he turns his back

  on real ity and truth.”18

  After Solomon Asch came Stanley Milgram. Milgram’s first

  famous study was, like Asch’s experiments, a study of conformity.

  He asked some of his students to stand on a sidewalk, looking

  at a building’s win dow, and counted how many of the people

  passing by would imitate them.19 When enough students were

  looking in the same direction— the critical group size seemed

  to be about five— nearly all those who passed by followed the

  students in looking at the building. It was as if people could not

  help but follow the crowd.

  But Milgram is best known for a later, much more provoca-

  tive experiment.20 In this study, participants were asked to take

  t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 7

  part in research bearing ostensibly on learning. In the lab, they

  were introduced to another participant— who, once again, was

  actually a confederate. The experimenter pretended to randomly

  pick one of the two— always the confederate—to be the learner.

  Participants were then told the study tested whether someone

  who was motivated to avoid electric shocks would learn better.

  The learner had to memorize a list of words; when he made a

  mistake, the participant would be asked to administer an elec-

  tric shock.

  The participants sat in front of a big machine with a series of

  switches corresponding to electric shocks of increasingly high

  voltage. The confederate was led slightly away, to an experimen-

  tal booth, but the participants could still hear him through a

  microphone. At first, the confederate did a good enough job

  memorizing the words, but as the task grew more difficult, he

  started making mistakes. The experimenter prompted the par-

  ticipants to shock the confederate, and all of them did. This was

  hardly surprising, as the first switches were marked as deliver-

  ing only a “slight shock.” As the confederate kept making

  mistakes, the experimenter urged the participants to increase the

  voltage. The switches went from “slight shock,” to “moderate

  shock,” then “strong shock,” and “very strong shock,” yet all the

  participants kept flipping the switches. It was only on the last

  switch of the “intense shock” series—300 volts— that a few par-

  ticipants refused to proceed. All the while, the confederate ex-

  pressed his discomfort. At some point, he started howling in

  pain, begging the participants to stop: “Let me out of here! You

  can’t hold me here! Get me out of here!”21 He even complained

  of heart prob lems. Yet the vast majority of participants kept

  going.

  When the “extreme intensity shock” series began, a few more

  participants stopped. One participant refused to go on when the

  8 ch ap t er 1

  switches indicated “danger: severe shock.” At this stage, the con-

  federate had simply stopped screaming and was begging to be

  freed. He then became completely unresponsive. But that didn’t

  stop two- thirds of the participants from flipping the last two

  switches, 435 volts and 450 volts, marked with an ominous

  “XXX.” Milgram had gotten a substantial majority of these or-

  dinary American citizens to deliver (what they thought to be)

  potentially lethal electric shocks to a fellow citizen who (they

  thought) was writhing in pain and begging for mercy.

  When learning of these results, and of a litany of historical

  cases seemingly attesting to similar phenomena, it is hard not to

  agree with the sweeping indictment leveled by po liti cal phi los-

  o pher Jason
Brennan: “ Human beings are wired not to seek truth

  and justice but to seek consensus. They are shackled by social

  pressure. They are overly deferential to authority. They cower

  before uniform opinion. They are swayed not so much by rea-

  son but by a desire to belong, by emotional appeal, and by sex

  appeal.”22 Psychologist Daniel Gilbert and his colleagues con-

  cur: “That human beings are, in fact, more gullible than they are

  suspicious should prob ably ‘be counted among the first and most

  common notions that are innate in us.’ ”23

  If you believe that humans are by nature credulous, the natu-

  ral question to ask is: Why? Already in 500 BCE Heraclitus, one

  of the first recorded Greek phi los o phers, was wondering:

  What use are the people’s wits

  who let themselves be led

  by speechmakers, in crowds,

  without considering

  how many fools and thieves

  they are among, and how few

  choose the good?24

  t he c a se f o r g ul l ib il i t y 9

  Heraclitus was echoed twenty- five hundred years later in a less

  poetic but more concise manner by this headline from the BBC:

  “Why are people so incredibly gullible?”25

  Adaptive Credulity

  If social psychologists seem to have been bent on demonstrat-

  ing human credulity, anthropologists have, for the most part,

  taken it for granted.26 Many have seen the per sis tence of tradi-

  tional beliefs and be hav iors as unproblematic: children simply

  imbibe the culture that surrounds them, thereby ensuring its

  continuity. Logically, anthropologists have devoted little at-

  tention to children, who are supposed to be mere receptacles for

  the knowledge and skil s of the previous generation.27 Critical

  anthropologists have described the assumption that people

  absorb what ever culture surrounds them as the theory of “ex-

  haustive cultural transmission,”28 or, more pejoratively, as the

  “ ‘fax model’ of internalization.”29

  For all its simplicity, this model of cultural transmission helps

  us understand why people would be credulous: so they learn the

  knowledge and skil s acquired by generations of their ancestors.

  Biologist Richard Dawkins thus explains the “programmed-in

  gullibility of a child” by its “useful[ness] for learning language

  and traditional wisdom.”30

  While it is easy to think of “traditional wisdom” one would

 

‹ Prev