by Hugo Mercier
versity of Chicago Press.
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Silk, J. B. (1995). “The role of grunts in reconciling
opponents and facilitating interactions among adult female baboons.” Animal
Behaviour, 50(1), 249–257.
Chiang, C.- F., & Knight, B. (2011). “Media bias and influence: Evidence from news-
paper endorsements.” Review of Economic Studies, 78(3), 795–820.
Chiarel a, S. S., & Poulin- Dubois, D. (2013). “Cry babies and Pollyannas: Infants can
detect unjustified emotional reactions.” Infancy, 18(s1), E81– E96.
Chiarel a, S. S., & Poulin- Dubois, D. (2015). “ ‘Aren’t you supposed to be sad?’ Infants
do not treat a stoic person as an unreliable emoter.” Infant Be hav ior and Develop-
ment, 38, 57–66.
Choleris, E., Guo, C., Liu, H., Mainardi, M., & Valsecchi, P. (1997). “The effect of dem-
onstrator age and number on duration of socially- induced food preferences in
house mouse ( Mus domesticus).” Behavioural Pro cesses, 41(1), 69–77.
Chorus, A. (1953). “The basic law of rumor.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy chol-
ogy, 48(2), 313–314.
Chwe, M. (2001). Rational ritual. New York: Prince ton University Press.
Claidière, N., Trouche, E., & Mercier, H. (2017). “Argumentation and the diffusion
of counter- intuitive beliefs.” Journal of Experimental Psy chol ogy: General, 146(7),
1052–1066.
Clauset, A., Arbesman, S., & Larremore, D. B. (2015). “Systematic in equality and hi-
erarchy in faculty hiring networks.” Science Advances, 1(1), e1400005.
Clément, F. (2006). Les mécanismes de la crédulité. Geneva: Librairie Droz.
Clément, F. (2010). “To trust or not to trust? Children’s social epistemology.” Review
of Philosophy and Psy chol ogy, 1(4), 1–19.
r e f e r e n c e s 315
Clément, F., Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. (2004). “The ontogenesis of trust.” Mind and
Language, 19(4), 360–379.
Cohen, I. B. (1985). Revolution in science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cohn, N. (1970). The pursuit of the millennium. St. Albans: Paladin.
Col ins, P. J., Hahn, U., von Gerber, Y., & Olsson, E. J. (2018). “The bi- directional re-
lationship between source characteristics and message content.” Frontiers in Psy-
chol ogy, 9. Retrieved from https:// www .frontiersin .org / articles / 10 .3389 / fpsyg .2018
. 00018 / full
Condorcet, J. A. N. (1785). Essai sur l’application de l’analyse à la probabilité des déci-
sions rendues à la pluralité des voix.
Condorcet, J. A. N. (1797). Esquisse d’un tableau historique des progrès de l’esprit
humain.
Conner, A. W. (2000). “True confessions? Chinese confessions then and now.” In K. G.
Turner, J. V. Feinerman, & R. K. Guy (Eds.), The limits of the rule of law in China
(pp. 132–162). Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Conradt, L., & List, C. (2009). “Group decisions in humans and animals: A survey.”
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences,
364(1518), 719–742.
Conradt, L., & Roper, T. J. (2003). “Group decision- making in animals.” Nature,
421(6919), 155–158.
Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2010). “Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the ma-
jority in making simple perceptual judgments.” Developmental Psy chol ogy, 46(2),
437–445.
Costas- Pérez, E., Solé- Ollé, A., & Sorribas- Navarro, P. (2012). “Corruption scandals,
voter information, and accountability.” Eu ro pean Journal of Po liti cal Economy, 28(4),
469–484.
Couch, C. J. (1968). “Collective be hav ior: An examination of some ste reo types.” So-
cial Prob lems, 15(3), 310–322.
Couil ard, N. L., & Woodward, A. L. (1999). “ Children’s comprehension of deceptive
points.” British Journal of Developmental Psy chol ogy, 17(4), 515–521.
Coviello, L., Sohn, Y., Kramer, A. D., Marlow, C., Franceschetti, M., Christakis, N. A.,
& Fowler, J. H. (2014). “Detecting emotional contagion in massive social net-
works.” PloS One, 9(3), e90315.
Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). “Facial displays are tools for social influence.”
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(5), 388–399.
Crowel , A., & Kuhn, D. (2014). “Developing dialogic argumentation skil s: A 3- year
intervention study.” Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 363–381.
Davies, S. R. (1997). Popu lar opinion in Stalin’s Rus sia: Terror, propaganda and dissent,
1934–1941. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dawkins, R. (2010). A dev il’s chaplain: Selected writings. London: Hachette UK.
316 references
Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). “Animal signals: Information or manipulation?”
In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecol ogy: An evolutionary approach
(pp. 282–309). Oxford: Basil Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Dediu, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2018). “Neanderthal language revisited: Not only us.”
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 49–55.
Dehaene, S. (1999). The number sense: How the mind creates mathe matics. Oxford: Ox-
ford University Press.
Del aVigna, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2010). “Persuasion: Empirical evidence.” Annual Re-
view of Economics, 2(1), 643–669.
Del aVigna, S., & Kaplan, E. (2007). “The Fox News effect: Media bias and voting.”
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(3), 1187–1234.
Delton, A. W., & Cimino, A. (2010). “Exploring the evolved concept of NEWCOMER:
Experimental tests of a cognitive model.” Evolutionary Psy chol ogy, 8(2),
147470491000800220.
Delumeau, J. (1977). Catholicism between Luther and Voltaire. Philadelphia: Westmin-
ster Press.
Del Vicario, M., Scala, A., Caldarel i, G., Stanley, H. E., & Quattrociocchi, W. (2017).
“Modeling confirmation bias and polarization.” Scientific Reports, 7, 40391.
Demick, B. (2010). Nothing to envy: Real lives in North Korea. New York: Spiegel and
Grau.
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. London: Penguin Books.
DePaulo, B. M. (1992). “Nonverbal be hav ior and self- presentation.” Psychological Bul-
letin, 111(2), 203–243.
DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein, J. A. (1996).
“Lying in everyday life.” Journal of Personality and Social Psy chol ogy, 70(5),
979–995.
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Coo-
per, H. (2003). “Cues to deception.” Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 74–118.
Desmet, K., & Wacziarg, R. (2018). The cultural divide. NBER Working Paper
No. 24630. Retrived from https:// www .nber .org / papers / w24630
Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). “Two perspectives on pointing in
infancy.” In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in
development (pp. 85–101). Hil sdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dessalles, J.- L. (2007). Why we talk: The evolutionary origins of language. Cambridge:
Oxford University Press.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). “A study of normative and informational social
influences upon individual judgment.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy chol ogy,
51(3)
, 629–636.
De Vries, C. E., Hobolt, S. B., & Tilley, J. (2018). “Facing up to the facts: What causes
economic perceptions?” Electoral Studies, 51, 115–122.
r e f e r e n c e s 317
de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics. New York: Harper and Row.
Dewitt, S. H., Lagnado, D., & Fenton, N. E. (submitted). Updating prior beliefs based
on ambiguous evidence. Retrieved from https:// www .researchgate .net / publication
/ 326610460 _ Updating _ Prior _ Beliefs _ Based _ on _ Ambiguous _ Evidence
Dezecache, G. (2015). “ Human collective reactions to threat.” Wiley Interdisciplinary
Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(3), 209–219.
Dezecache, G., Martin, J. R., Tessier, C., Safra, L., Pitron, V., Nuss, P., & Grèzes, J. (sub-
mitted). Social strategies in response to deadly danger during a mass shooting.
Dezecache, G., Mercier, H., & Scott- Phil ips, T. C. (2013). “An evolutionary approach
to emotional communication.” Journal of Pragmatics, 59(B), 221–233.
Dickson, G. (2007). The Children’s Crusade: Medieval history, modern mythistory. Lon-
don: Palgrave Macmil an.
DiFonzo, N. (2010). “Ferreting facts or fashioning fallacies? Factors in rumor accu-
racy.” Social and Personality Psy chol ogy Compass, 4(11), 1124–1137.
DiFonzo, N., & Bordia, P. (2007). Rumor psy chol ogy: Social and orga nizational ap-
proaches. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Diggory, J. C. (1956). “Some consequences of proximity to a disease threat.” Sociom-
etry, 19(1), 47–53.
Dikötter, F. (2010). Mao’s great famine: The history of China’s most devastating catastro-
phe, 1958–1962. New York: Walker and Com pany.
Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., & Elmehed, K. (2000). “Unconscious facial reactions to
emotional facial expressions.” Psychological Science, 11(1), 86–89.
Dixon, G., Hmielowski, J., & Ma, Y. (2017). “Improving climate change ac cep tance
among US conservatives through value- based message targeting.” Science Com-
munication, 39(4), 520–534.
Dockendorff, M., & Mercier, H. (in preparation). Argument transmission as the weak
link in the correction of po liti cal misbeliefs.
Donovan, P. (2004). No way of knowing: Crime, urban legends and the internet. Lon-
don: Routledge.
Drizin, S. A., & Leo, R. A. (2003). “The prob lem of false confessions in the post- DNA
world.” North Carolina Law Review, 82, 891–1007.
Druckman, J. N., Levendusky, M. S., & McLain, A. (2018). “No need to watch: How
the effects of partisan media can spread via interpersonal discussions.” American
Journal of Po liti cal Science, 62(1), 99–112.
Drury, J., Novel i, D., & Stott, C. (2013). “Psychological disaster myths in the percep-
tion and management of mass emergencies.” Journal of Applied Social Psy chol ogy,
43(11), 2259–2270.
Dubois, E., & Blank, G. (2018). “The echo chamber is overstated: The moderating ef-
fect of po liti cal interest and diverse media.” Information, Communication and
Society, 21(5), 729–745.
318 references
Dubreuil, B. (2010). “Paleolithic public goods games: Why human culture and coop-
eration did not evolve in one step.” Biology and Philosophy, 25(1), 53–73.
Dumont, L. (1980). Homo hierarchicus: The caste system and its implications. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Dunlap, A. S., Nielsen, M. E., Dornhaus, A., & Papaj, D. R. (2016). “Foraging bumble
bees weigh the reliability of personal and social information.” Current Biology,
26(9), 1195–1199.
Durante, R., & Gutierrez, E. (2014). Po liti cal advertising and voting intentions: Evidence
from exogenous variation in ads viewership. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from
https:// spire .sciencespo .fr / hdl: / 2441 / 26lctatf2u813of8nkn7j2230h / resources
/ wp - mexico - political - advertising .pdf.
Durbach, N. (2000). “ ‘They might as well brand us’: Working- class re sis tance to compul-
sory vaccination in Victorian England.” Social History of Medicine, 13(1), 45–63.
Ebrahim, G. J. (1968). “Mass hysteria in school children: Notes on three outbreaks
in East Africa.” Clinical Pediatrics, 7(7), 437–438.
Ecker, U. K., O’Reilly, Z., Reid, J. S., & Chang, E. P. (2019). The effectiveness of short-
format refutational fact- checks. British Journal of Psy chol ogy. https:// doi .org / 10
. 1111 / bjop .12383.
Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). “A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of argu-
ments.” Journal of Personality and Social Psy chol ogy, 71(1), 5–24.
Einav, S. (2017). “Thinking for themselves? The effect of in for mant in de pen dence
on children’s endorsement of testimony from a consensus.” Social Development,
27(1), 73–86.
Einav, S., & Robinson, E. J. (2011). “When being right is not enough: Four- year- olds
distinguish knowledgeable in for mants from merely accurate in for mants.” Psycho-
logical Science, 22(10), 1250–1253.
Ekelund, R. B., Tollison, R. D., Anderson, G. M., Hébert, R. F., & Davidson, A. B.
(1996). Sacred trust: The medieval church as an economic firm. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Ekman, P. (2001). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage.
New York: Norton
Ekman, P. (2009). “Lie catching and microexpressions.” In C. Martin (Ed.), The phi-
losophy of deception (pp.118–133). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Enders, A. M., & Armaly, M. T. (2018). “The differential effects of actual and perceived
polarization.” Po liti cal Be hav ior, https:// doi .org / 10 .1007 / s11109 - 018 - 9476 - 2.
Engelberg, J. W., & Gouzoules, H. (2019). “The credibility of acted screams: Implica-
tions for emotional communication research.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psy chol ogy, 72(8), 1889–1902.
Epler, D. C. (1980). “Bloodletting in early Chinese medicine and its relation to the
origin of acu punc ture.” Bulletin of the History of Medicine, 54(3), 337–367.
r e f e r e n c e s 319
Eriksson, K. (2012). “The nonsense math effect.” Judgment and Decision Making, 7(6),
746–749.
Estlund, D. (1994). “Opinion leaders, in de pen dence, and Condorcet’s jury theorem.”
Theory and Decision, 36(2), 131–162.
Evans, H., & Bartholomew, R. (2009). Outbreak! The encyclopedia of extraordinary so-
cial be hav ior. New York: Anomalist Books.
Evans- Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, magic and oracles among the Azande. Retrieved
from eHRAF: World Cultures database.
Faasse, K., Chatman, C. J., & Martin, L. R. (2016). “A comparison of language use in
pro- and anti- vaccination comments in response to a high profile Facebook post.”
Vaccine, 34(47), 5808–5814.
Facchini, G., Margalit, Y., & Nakata, H. (2016). Countering public opposition to immi-
gration: The impact of information campaigns. Unpublished article. Retrieved from
https:// papers .ssrn .com / sol3 / papers .cfm ? abstract _ id = 2887349.
Fadda, M., Al am, A., & Schulz, P. J. (2015). “Arguments and sources on Italian online
forums on childhood vaccinations: Results of a content analy sis.” Vaccine, 33(51),
7152–7159.
Fershtman, C., &
Gneezy, U. (2001). “Discrimination in a segmented society: An ex-
perimental approach.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1), 351–377.
Fifield, A. (2019). The Great Successor: The divinely perfect destiny of brilliant comrade
Kim Jong Un. New York: PublicAffairs.
Fiorina, M. P., Abrams, S. J., & Pope, J. (2005). Culture war? The myth of a polarized
Amer ica. New York: Pearson Longman.
FitzGibbon, C. D., & Fanshawe, J. H. (1988). “Stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: An hon-
est signal of condition.” Behavioral Ecol ogy and Sociobiology, 23(2), 69–74.
Flaxman, S., Goel, S., & Rao, J. M. (2016). “Filter bubbles, echo chambers, and online
news consumption.” Public Opinion Quarterly, 80(S1), 298–320.
Fletcher, R., & Nielsen, R. K. (2017). “Are news audiences increasingly fragmented?
A cross- national comparative analy sis of cross- platform news audience fragmen-
tation and duplication.” Journal of Communication, 67(4), 476–498.
Foddy, M., Platow, M. J., & Yamagishi, T. (2009). “Group- based trust in strangers: The
role of ste reo types and expectations.” Psychological Science, 20(4), 419–422.
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Foerster, A., Wirth, R., Herbort, O., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2017). “Lying upside-
down: Alibis reverse cognitive burdens of dishonesty.” Journal of Experimental Psy-
chol ogy: Applied, 23(3), 301–319.
Fourney, A., Racz, M. Z., Ranade, G., Mobius, M., & Horvitz, E. (2017). “Geographic
and temporal trends in fake news consumption during the 2016 US presidential
election.” Proceedings of the 2017 ACM Conference on Information and Knowledge
Management, 2071–2074.
320 references
Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of emotions. New York:
Norton.
Frederick, S. (2005). “Cognitive reflection and decision making.” Journal of Economic
Perspectives, 19(4), 25–42.
Fresco, N. (1980). “Les redresseurs de morts. Chambres à gaz: la bonne nouvelle.”
Comment on révise l’histoire. Les Temps Modernes, 407, 2150–2211.
Freud, S. (1905). “Fragment of an analy sis of a case of hysteria.” In E. Jones (Ed.), Col-
lected papers (pp. 13–146). New York: Basic Books.
Friend, R., Rafferty, Y., & Bramel, D. (1990). “A puzzling misinterpretation of the Asch