Why Faith Fails The Christian Delusion

Home > Other > Why Faith Fails The Christian Delusion > Page 9
Why Faith Fails The Christian Delusion Page 9

by John W. Loftus

belief and/or information that brings the new evidence into question are the

  quickest methods to eliminate their cognitive dissonance.20

  People simply become increasingly sure of their decisions after they have

  made them by rationalizing their choice of alternatives, which serves to reduce

  the cognitive dissonance produced by foregoing the good features of the

  unchosen alternative and accepting the bad features of the chosen alternative.21

  When it comes to religion, a believer will defend his faith and attack the

  alternatives in part simply because he has already rendered a decision on the

  matter. Furthermore-and this is where the strength of the motivation kicks into

  overdrive-Petty and Cacioppo explain that the effects of cognitive dissonance

  increase as the positions between the two beliefs diverge and the perceived

  importance of establishing a position grows.22 Could any two positions be in

  sharper contrast than the existence and nonexistence of God? Could any

  dilemma be more important to the Christian than whether or not God exists? It

  naturally follows that questions on the issue of God's existence provoke the most

  cognitive dissonance within those who are deeply involved in the issue. As this

  debate generates the greatest amount of cognitive dissonance, it naturally

  follows that people are increasingly willing to accept explanations that alleviate

  the uncomfortable feelings and are decreasingly willing to consider

  disconfirming arguments. As the uneasiness becomes more powerful, people

  become more willing to surrender to whatever arguments are offered-just as

  when hunger becomes more powerful, people become more willing to eat

  whatever food is available. This will subsequently lead to accepting highly

  illogical justifications for maintaining highly important beliefs.

  A troubled Christian might not peruse, comprehend, or even read an entire

  argument offered in defense of his belief, but the mere fact that a possible

  answer exists satisfies him that there is a reasonable answer to the skeptical

  objection. Never mind the fact that anyone can cite an authority who agrees with

  a particular position, especially when it comes to interpreting religion. Due to an

  innate bias to confirm what we already believe, the authority's position is not

  going to be scrutinized or tested against a rebuttal. The Christian is interested in

  feeling comfortable with his beliefs, not in dispassionately evaluating them.

  People want to feel reassured that they are correct in their beliefs, especially

  when there is a lot of emotion, personality, history, and identity at stake. If the

  Christian were genuinely interested in the truth, he would analyze the argument

  critically and thoroughly to see if it adequately addressed the points of the

  skeptical objection. But he is not questioning; he is defending.

  There have been several prominent cases in which religious followers have

  manufactured outlandish explanations to relieve their cognitive dissonance.

  Members of a Southwestern United States evangelical cult who retreated into a

  bomb shelter to avoid a predicted imminent nuclear apocalypse went on to

  believe, after it didn't occur, that it did not take place because of their prayers!23

  Members of the Keech cult, who were to be taken to the planet Clarion to avoid

  a global flood, went on to believe afterward, when it didn't happen, that aliens

  told their leader it was prevented by their faith.24 Mormons believe Joseph

  Smith's unwillingness to retranslate the lost portions of the Book of Mormon was

  forbidden by God, even though it's crystal clear to the rest of us that Smith just

  couldn't remember verbatim what he had improvised and dictated. Christians

  believe that Jesus' failed prophecies of his return in his very near future aren't

  what they truly appear to be-failed predictions-as Loftus shows in chapter 12 of

  this book. These kinds of post hoc rationalistic justifications relieve the

  uncomfortable dissonance generated after external elements showed the facts

  were inconsistent with their beliefs.

  In addition to cognitive dissonance theory, there are two complementary

  theories that help explain the reason why people provide illogical defenses for

  their beliefs. Impression management theory suggests that people increasingly

  stick by their decisions because consistency leads to social reward and

  inconsistency leads to social punishment.25 Psychological reactance theory

  suggests that people increasingly stick by their decisions when others threaten

  their freedom to express their ideas.26 It is my opinion that limited persecution

  in Rome during the infant years of Christianity may have dramatically increased

  its popularity. It's not difficult to imagine how people would become more

  dedicated to and firm in their beliefs when faced with opposition, especially

  when the opposition pushes a sharp reversal of current conditions.'? People

  within the threatened group respond by dropping their differences to unite for a

  common cause, while people outside the group desire what the authorities

  forbade. There have been several historical instances in which the abolishment

  of certain products, services, and rights has led to an increase in the desire for

  those products, services, and rights.28

  When religious believers are willing to admit that people often believe what they

  are raised to believe and that people are often incapable of rational thinking due

  to the effects of cognitive dissonance, they will often fall back to utilizing the

  arguments from experts who agree with them. However, if you wanted safety

  information on a used car, would it be wiser to trust the word of a used car

  salesperson or the findings of a consumer report? Similarly, if you wanted to

  obtain information on the historicity and veracity of Islam, would you ask only

  an Islamic scholar who has been taught about Islamic sanctity since childhood,

  or would you also ask a secular scholar with no emotional investment in Islam?

  Would you not also do the same for Hinduism, Mormonism, Buddhism, and so

  on? If you utilize the same reasoning and choose the unbiased scholar to help

  evaluate that position, as you very well should, why make an exception only for

  Christianity? People who study a concept in which they have no emotional

  investment are probably going to offer more reliable conclusions than those who

  want the concept to yield a specific result.

  Scholars who begin with no emotional investment in Christianity probably

  present the most unbiased conclusions about it simply because they are more

  open during their studies to accept evidence that contradicts their tentative

  conclusions. Just as the used car salesperson will be hesitant to acknowledge and

  relay information that is damaging to the quality of his vehicles, the Christian

  scholar will be hesitant to acknowledge and relay information that is damaging

  to the veracity of his religion. We have no reason to think that belief in

  Christianity provides a special insight into the veracity of it, because every

  religion can make a parallel claim. The opinions of individuals with emotional

  involvement, personal attachments, or vested interests in the outcome of a

  debatable issue are less
likely to change when confronted with new information

  because people have an innate inclination to seek evidence that confirms their

  preestablished beliefs and to ignore evidence that will not. Psychologists call this

  confirmation bias. We often do not weigh the facts before making judgments, but

  rather we believe in things based on our predispositions and influences. We then

  maintain these beliefs by ignoring and rationalizing away evidence that

  contradicts our preexisting conclusions. We believe in things for reasons other

  than logical reasoning and empirical evidence.

  When subjects are given the task of solving a problem, most will immediately

  form a hypothesis and only look for examples to confirm it. They do not seek

  evidence to disprove the hypothesis, and they are very slow to change the

  hypothesis even when it is obviously wrong. Subjects also adopt overly simple

  hypotheses or strategies for solutions if the information is too complex, and form

  hypotheses about coincidental relationships they observe if there is no true

  solution.29 By adopting these overly simple hypotheses and strategies for

  complex issues, individuals gain immediate gratification. Subjects will then

  ignore, distort, and eventually forget evidence for theories that they do not

  prefer. As the subjects spend more time focusing on internally justifying their

  beliefs, the confidence in their beliefs greatly increases.30

  Psychologists Muzafer Sherif and Carl Hovland have demonstrated that a

  person's level of emotional involvement with an issue has an enormous impact

  on how new evidence is interpreted.31 Involved persons have beliefs with

  greater specificity and therefore have larger latitudes of rejection for evidence

  that does not fit with those beliefs. People with a high involvement are more

  resistant to contrary persuasion than less involved persons because any given

  message has a greater probability of falling into the rejection region.

  Psychologist Drew Westen was among those who empirically demonstrated,

  using MRI scanning, that people who were strongly loyal to one candidate in

  presidential elections did not use areas of the brain associated with reasoning to

  resolve contradictory statements made by their candidate. The supporters instead

  relied upon regions of the brain associated with emotion to justify their personal

  allegiances.32 I could cite numerous similar studies that demonstrate irrational

  behavior from highly involved individuals, but this is sufficient to establish my

  point that people shun dispassionate critical thought when justifying their most

  important beliefs and personal values.

  Our analysis of emotionally involved people should lead us to an important

  question in desperate need of an answer. What good is a biblical scholar who

  refuses to consider that his point of view may simply be wrong? If past research

  tells us there are three scientific procedures capable of yielding a cure for a

  hypothetical disease, would we ever trust a scientist who was indoctrinated since

  childhood to believe that only one of those three procedures could produce a

  cure? Should we honestly believe that a biblical apologist who began with the

  notion of an inspired Bible would readily consider the possibility that his holy

  book is fundamentally flawed? Many of the top Christian apologists even admit

  that when the data conflicts with the text, we should trust the text. Prominent

  apologist William Lane Craig declares, "[S]hould a conflict arise between the

  witness of the Holy Spirit to the fundamental truth of the Christian faith and

  beliefs based on argument and evidence, then it is the former which must take

  precedence over the latter, not vice versa."33 One of the most widely touted

  Young Earth Creationist Web sites, Answersln Genesis.org, even offers this in

  their statement of faith: "No apparent, perceived, or claimed evidence in any

  field, including history and chronology, can be valid if it contradicts the

  Scriptural record."34 What's the point in listening to people like this? Such is the

  problem with all religious apologists, regardless of the specific belief. They will

  begin by presuming certain premises are true and mold explanations to patch the

  apparent problems, no matter how insulting the explanations are to common

  sense. This is how religions thrive in the age of scrutiny and reason. Apologists

  will find a "resolution" to every objection. No skeptical author can offer

  anything that Christian apologists think they cannot answer. After all, God wrote

  it, so it must be true-even if it violates science and common sense. Misguided

  believers often accomplish this intellectually dishonest defense by citing one of

  these biblical authorities who have also been indoctrinated, probably to an even

  greater degree.

  The focus we need to place on apologetic defenses is the likelihood of the

  offered explanations and how an unbiased, dispassionate individual would rule

  on these explanations. Is the apologetic suggestion a likely solution to the

  problem, or is it a way of maintaining predetermined beliefs? Since most staunch

  Bible defenders have already declared that nothing is going to change their

  minds (and the solutions to presented biblical complications often reflect this

  disposition), we must be highly suspicious of the intellectual honesty put forth

  toward apologetic solutions. After all, there are even apologists for contradictory

  schools of thought within Christianity itself. How can two groups of people

  consistently use two contradictory avenues of thought yet consistently arrive at

  the same answer, unless the conclusion itself consistently preceded the

  explanation? A dispassionate outlook is an indispensable necessity when in

  search of the truth. Religious scholars who began as religious believers lack this

  critical component. The practice of religion clouds judgment; understanding of

  religion does not.

  People who have an interest in pursuing a career in Christianity are

  undoubtedly those who have already been indoctrinated with the importance of

  it. If they believe in Christianity ardently enough to pursue a career from it, they

  are unquestionably more likely to interpret evidence so that it is favorable to

  their preconceived notions. So it should come as no surprise that the vast

  majority of experts in any religion believe in the very religion that they study. A

  majority of experts in the history of the ancient Near East will defend positions

  that are beneficial to Christianity precisely because they were born in a Christian

  society. The majority of those who will back the Qur'an were born in an Islamic

  society. The majority of those who will back the Torah were born in a Jewish

  society. The opinions of these authorities, who began with a certain conclusion

  instead of analyzing the evidence to reach that conclusion, cannot be trusted

  merely because they are authorities. Conclusions based upon evidence are

  important. Conclusions based upon evidence that has been interpreted to support

  an a priori assumption are not. For these reasons, I put little stock in the opinions

  of people who began studying Christianity years after they settled on the

  existence of a talking donkey.
<
br />   If an intelligent, rational group of people who were never exposed to the idea

  of religion were asked to become experts in the history of the ancient Near East,

  the unanimous consensus of the group would be that the Bible is bunk. They

  would reach this conclusion for two reasons: there is absolutely nothing in the

  book that would impress critically thinking dispassionate outsiders, and they

  would not have been exposed to the centuries of aura and mystique that society

  has placed on the Bible. To them, the Bible would not be a guide that billions

  have used for worship; it would be just another book in the mythology section of

  the library.

  Are there some who are more likely than others to leave the comfortable

  confines of religious thought? Petty and Cacioppo point out that people with low

  self-esteem are more likely to accept messages that confirm an initial viewpoint,

  and less likely to be persuaded away from it.3' The very foundation of

  Christianity is built upon the suggestion that we are insignificant creatures

  compared to the creator of the universe and that it is not possible to carry out a

  meaningful existence without accepting the biblical belief system.36 However,

  once we accept the biblical teachings (and only after doing so), we become

  worthy of God's gift of eternal life. Such ideas are no doubt appealing to those

  with little or no self-esteem, but they carry less weight with someone confident

  of his own abilities and intelligence. Speaking of which, as Paul Bell of Mensa

  reports, "of 43 studies carried out since 1927, on the relationship between

  religious belief and one's intelligence or educational level, all but four found an

  inverse connection," and even of those four none indicated anything to the

  contrary. In other words, the higher your intelligence or educational level, the

  less likely you are to be religious.37 Intelligence and educational achievement is

  therefore another factor affecting who will find religious belief unpersuasive.

  Psychologist Frank Sulloway has shown that people with open minds also

  compose one group less likely to be religious.38 This conclusion might seem

  counterintuitive, especially considering how mystical ideas are commonly

  purported to reveal themselves to those with "open minds," but the results make

  sense upon further reflection. Skeptics have their positions but are willing to

  consider other viewpoints; the religious are indoctrinated not to seriously

  question their beliefs. It does not take a willfully open mind to accept the

  existence of God because it is essentially the default position in our culture. It

  does take an open mind, however, to consider the possibility that one's most

  sacred beliefs might be false.

  While it's true there are still plenty of highly intelligent people who are

  religious, we should ask ourselves why this is so. The best answer, in my

  opinion, comes from Shermer, within the very argument that he became famous

  for coining: "Smart people believe weird things because they are skilled at

  defending beliefs they arrived at for non-smart reasons."39 Believing in

  otherwise absurd stories simply because they are part of a religion bestowed

  upon you obviously qualifies as believing in something for "non-smart reasons."

  The intellectual meltdown arrives from gifted people inventing extremely clever

  (but equally absurd) reasons why they think their beliefs are correct. Speaking

  from personal experience, I can say that even in the confines of solidarity, I

  could not be realistic about my former beliefs for one key reason: It is never easy

  to be honest with yourself about the Bible when a mind-reading god is always

  present. Simply thinking that God did something wrong might be as

 

‹ Prev