The Book of Memory

Home > Other > The Book of Memory > Page 25
The Book of Memory Page 25

by Mary Carruthers


  No one will have any difficulty in recognizing this counsel to be at once

  typically medieval and, from a modern standpoint, bizarre to the point of

  intellectual decadence. Yet the scheme proposed is eminently sensible as a

  mnemonic both for composing and for delivering a sermon, as well as

  providing auditors with a clear structure within which they can grasp and

  retain the major points of the sermon.

  This example, like many others, combines a variety of mnemonic

  structures, which in this case are also reflected in its compositional struc-

  tures. 99 The theme is the Passion, and the preacher elects to divide by fives,

  a number with evident associations to his subject because there were five

  wounds in the crucifixion. A mnemonic rhyme composed of summary

  catchphrases organizes the main division in five. Then, in the subdivision,

  one remembers the five wounds, imposing upon the structure of those five

  another five, the five vowels, or voces, perhaps used because of the word’s

  punning association with voce magna, the third division of the text which

  deals with the theme humanitatis veritas. (Punning of all sorts is funda-

  mental in mnemotechnic; one encounters visualized homophony through-

  out the history of written mnemonic advice, as a principle both for forming

  images and for association of ideas.) To each of the five vowels is attached a

  text beginning with it. Five vowels, five wounds; the basic composition is

  complete, combining a scheme of loci and imagines that incorporates a

  Elementary memory design

  133

  numerical grid, on which are ‘‘placed’’ brief texts and other sorts of visual

  images. As hermeneutic, an interpretation of their meaning, attaching

  these particular texts to the five vowels is grotesque non-sense, but as an

  elementary device for retaining and recollecting them – as heuristic – it is

  both simple and effective. One can return to the theme of one’s compo-

  sition after a detailed exposition of any of its pieces and readily find one’s

  place.

  Robert of Basevorn’s other examples follow the same principles. For a

  division into six of the theme, ‘‘Ego vox clamantis in deserto, parate viam

  Domini,’’ he suggests using the six basic syllables of the chant, a pun on

  ‘‘vox clamantis.’’ To these six notes – ut, re, mi, fa, sol, la – are attached the

  texts for the subdivision, as follows:

  1) UT filii lucis ambulate; 2) REvertere, revertere, sulamitis, revertere, revertere ut

  intueamur te; 3) MIsere animae tuae placens Deo; 4) FAcite dignos fructus

  poenitentiae; 5) SOLve vincula colli tui, captiva filia Syon; 6) LAvamini, mundi

  estote. 100

  This is a most revealing application of the technique called solmization, for

  it shows that Robert of Basevorn understood that the device was primarily a

  mnemonic, and could thus be utilized in non-musical contexts. It also

  suggests how much more broadly the function of a mnemonic was under-

  stood then than it is now, when we restrict it to reiterative tasks.

  The principle of attaching a syllable to a particular musical value or

  ‘‘degree’’ was known in antiquity, and indeed is found in virtually every

  culture. 101 It is most notably discussed in the Middle Ages in a letter of

  Guido d’Arezzo to the monk, Michael. There is evidence that solmization

  was used in medieval Europe before Guido, but the Benedictines were

  certainly happy to give him credit for having ‘‘invented’’ it, and the system

  he describes became the dominant one, indeed the foundation of what

  Westerners still use. Guido’s syllables are taken from a hymn to John the

  Baptist, ‘‘Ut queant laxis’’; these syllables then became the names of the six

  parts of a hexachord, at that time the unit organizing musical tones. As

  Guido wrote to Michael:

  You will then have an altogether easy and thoroughly tested method of finding an

  unknown melody . . . After I began teaching this procedure to boys, some of them

  were able before the third day to sing an unknown melody [to them] with ease,

  which by other methods would not have been possible in many weeks.102

  His system is designedly mnemonic, ‘‘if you wish to commit any note or

  neume to memory,’’103 whether you knew the melody before or not, ‘‘quem

  scias vel nescias.’’ For the system of places forms a kind of alphabet. Indeed,

  134

  The Book of Memory

  Guido’s analysis of the purpose and function of musical notation seems to

  derive from Isidore of Seville’s definition of the purpose of letters, that is, to

  hold things in memory, including what we do not already know. Just as the

  alphabet enables us to hear again and retain in memory the voices/words of

  those who are not actually present and whom we have never heard or seen

  in the flesh, so musical notation is, in this way, like an alphabet (and so says

  John of Salisbury, in a passage I will examine in the next part of this

  chapter).

  Here, as well as in any other medieval text, we can understand that craft

  rules (artes) were thought to be, as Aristotle says, built up from repeated,

  remembered experiences, the principle being to recognize and organize

  likeness. This could then be applied, even to things never seen before and

  encountered for the first time. This is not mnemonic in the restricted sense

  that moderns tend to understand it, but in the larger sense of how all

  learning takes place. Learning is regarded as a process of discovering

  more effective, efficient, inclusive mnemonics – for memory, as Hugh of

  St. Victor says, is the basis of learning. To learn the alphabet (and numbers

  were designated by the alphabet, too, we should keep in mind) was to be

  possessed of a set of infinitely rich mnemonics – not only a skill that would

  enable one to read signs on a piece of vellum, but a key opening the door to

  the whole cultural complex of institutionalized practices signified in the

  word memoria. This is an analysis of learning that differs from our own in

  its emphasis upon memory and memorial cues, and it is very far from

  restricted to rote tasks. It places rote in the service of creative thought.

  Whatever the number of one’s sermon division, Robert of Basevorn

  recommends attaching a set of symbols or markers that incorporate that

  number and can be used as a mnemonic for the subdivisions. For example,

  besides the two already described, Robert suggests the seven mercies

  of God, the eight Beatitudes, the nine orders of angels, the Ten

  Commandments, the twelve hours of the day. But eleven, he says, is almost

  never used, and never a number greater than twelve, for one will lose track.

  This observation is borne out in a catalogue of such numbered groups of

  items that was made anonymously and attached, as Book IV, to Hugh of

  Fouilloy’s De avibus. Though many threes and sevens, fours, sixes, tens,

  and nines are listed, there are only two elevens – and nothing greater than

  twelve. 104 It is also useful, Basevorn says, to break up a larger number; for

  example, subdividing an initial division of nine into three sets of three,

  since there are so many threes th
at finding them presents no problem.

  Mary and Richard Rouse comment, concerning a sermon of Alan of Lille

  on the Trinity, that it is ‘‘an extravaganza of triplicity’’ – so too they well

  Elementary memory design

  135

  characterize the sermons of Innocent III as amounting to ‘‘a sequence of

  lists’’ of threes or fives or fours or sevens. 105 The subject matters so grouped

  are the singulars of which Hugh of St. Victor speaks, sorted and stored like

  coins of similar size in the properly numbered compartments of the

  memorial store-house.

  T H E A L P H A B E T A N D K E Y - W O R D S Y S T E M

  The number grid which Hugh of St. Victor describes was but one of several

  memorial heuristics taught in the Middle Ages. Memory training, we learn

  from Quintilian, was an elementary component of learning to read and to

  write. The numerical coding seems best designed to remember and to

  collate (that is, move discrete pieces of texts about in various combinations)

  longer works, like the Psalms, although it clearly can be used for a variety of

  other purposes as well. But in addition to such a task, a memory library

  needs to have cross-referencing systems, and these also seem to have been

  taught among the educational basics.

  Quintilian speaks both of memorizing a text in (numerical) order, and

  of marking the passages one especially wants or needs to remember with

  mental notae. Notae are discussed twice in relation to memory training.

  First, describing the system of loci and imagines, Quintilian suggests

  imprinting in orderly progression a spacious house with many rooms,

  and then marking the items to be remembered by a nota, either an

  associative sign (an anchor to remind one of navigation, for instance) or

  a key-word, for even people who lose the thread of what they are saying can

  put their memory back on track with a single cue. These notae are then

  placed in the orderly series of rooms. He prefers, however, a simpler

  system, yet one still using notae. After one divides a text into short sections,

  and has repeated these to oneself two or three times and perhaps written

  them down as well, one then marks passages one especially needs to

  recollect with notae which one has invented for oneself.106 Martianus

  Capella some 400 years later also advises using notae to mark passages,

  and the word is found commonly in memory advice, and ubiquitously

  written out in the margins of manuscripts against passages that either the

  scribe or a later reader thought to be especially important for remembering.

  Notes and places

  It is unclear from Quintilian and other writers whether these notae or

  notulae are to be inscribed on the physical page or only in memory, as

  136

  The Book of Memory

  Hugh of St. Victor counsels; clearly some were made physically, for

  marginal marks do occur in manuscripts, copied by scribes to help users

  of the books. But since every reader is advised to make notae, most

  mnemonic signs must have been devised mentally, especially when books

  were used by many different readers over several generations. In manu-

  script margins from the twelfth century on, one finds commonly the word,

  nota, addressed to the reader. It is the imperative singular of the verb notare,

  ‘‘make a note,’’ and it points out an important or difficult passage that the

  reader might wish particularly to mark with a nota of some sort of his own

  to help remember it.

  Mental marking is mnemonically advantageous because each individual

  makes up his own system of notulae, his own filing system, and, as we know

  from Ad Herennium, one’s own notae and imagines are to be preferred to

  memorizing a pre-existent system, because such exercise stimulates the

  memory more fully and fixes it more securely. Hugh of St. Victor describes

  a completely mental system; Robert Holcot, in a text we will examine again

  later, says that as he reads he imagines a memory-image projected onto the

  particular text that he wishes to remember, and he gives detailed instruc-

  tions in his commentaries for making these. Yet the written manuscript

  containing them is a plain one, rubricated but without any drawing or

  decoration. Only in the margin, opposite each verbal picture, is written the

  single word pictura. Perhaps we might understand the word in this context

  not as a noun, but, on the analogy of nota, as a command to the reader that

  he is to make a picture from the written description, evidently in his mind. 107

  At the end of the English Middle Ages, Stephen Hawes writes of the

  orator tucking into his memory ‘‘sundry ymages,’’ not only for the ‘‘mater . . .

  lyke to the tale’ but also in a ‘ recapytulacyon’ (‘ re-chaptering’ or ‘ cross-

  heading’ ) of each image by its ‘‘moralyzacyon,’’ a general ethical topic or

  ‘ commonplace’ heading. Much material – how much would have to vary

  individually – seems to have been assumed to be filed away not as a

  complete text (or not only as a complete text) but as sets of extracts

  ‘‘noted’’ in the memory. The verb tractare, meaning to ‘‘draw out’’ or

  ‘‘extract,’ is frequently used of a process of reading as well as of a genre

  of composition. To ‘‘tract’’ a text while reading it means to pull out of it the

  various matters that one wishes to squirrel away in one’s memorial inven-

  tory. Then, when composing a tract (the gerund form, tractandum, is used

  to indicate this activity), one begins by collecting on a particular subject all

  the material, together with appropriate commentary, that one had previ-

  ously drawn out of one’s reading and filed away under a letter or a key-

  word or some other nota.

  Elementary memory design

  137

  A late but important description of memory-work by the fifteenth-

  century Italian jurist, Peter of Ravenna, who wrote one of the works on

  the art of memory most popular in the Renaissance, states that a well-

  trained memory is most like a book containing both text and glosses. ‘‘For

  I daily read all my lectures of Canon Law without a book; but if I should

  have a book before my eyes, I deliver the textual concordances [textum] and

  glosses from memory so that I should not seem to omit the least sylla-

  ble.’’108 Thus, at the end of the fifteenth century, when printed books had

  become almost common, a law professor might still lecture regularly

  without a book, and could supply his lecturing apparatus more accurately

  and completely, he says, from his memory than by depending on a book.

  Peter’s memory-places were arranged alphabetically: ‘‘on the nineteen

  letters of the alphabet I have placed twenty thousand extracts of both sorts

  of law.’’ I will come back to this passage shortly to discuss exactly how he

  says he did this, but for now I want to stress his use of the alphabet as his

  ordering device. He uses, he says, the regular alphabet, but also ‘‘human

  figures in place of the letters and thus living, vivid images.’’109 For the sake

  of vivid images, unusual ones of the sort memory can easily fix on, he can

  make use of a sort
of human alphabet to indicate the various letters. He

  even suggests using the forms of enticing women for such a purpose: ‘‘illae

  enim multum memoriam meam excitant,’’ ‘‘these greatly stimulate my

  memory,’’ though, he adds, this is not a technique for those who loathe

  women or who cannot control themselves.

  Evidence that alphabets were commonly used as a mnemonic ordering

  device is scattered but persistent in both ancient and early medieval books.

  Aristotle describes (in De memoria) the use of letters of the Greek alphabet

  as an ordinary method for ordering memorized material. But if one is using

  an alphabet to file lots of different topics, one might well need more than

  one set of such places to accommodate one’s material, lest one’s memory be

  overwhelmed by trying to crowd too much into one place (a persistent

  warning in memory texts). To avoid this, one needs more than one set of

  alphabets, containing different forms though having still a rigid, easily

  recoverable, order. That is what Peter of Ravenna was doing with his

  human alphabets.

  It is a curiosity of medieval books that tables of Greek, Hebrew, Coptic,

  runic, and even wholly imaginary alphabets are found side by side in a

  number of monastic manuscripts. It is also an insufficiently explained

  curiosity that books containing the Bestiary, moralized descriptions of

  animals both real and imagined, are found commonly in monastery

  libraries. Scholars have wondered what function such apparently puerile,

  138

  The Book of Memory

  unscholarly material might serve to justify its preservation. I will return to

  this question in Chapters 4 and 7, to make my case more fully, but my

  hypothesis is that they functioned not only to delight and intrigue medieval

  students but to provide them with mnemonically valuable heuristics,

  orderly foundations or sets of mnemonic loci, which can continue to

  have value throughout one’s education (a lifetime project). In this context,

  such a list of letters does not serve primarily to form words (and indeed in

  the case of imaginary alphabets, no words are to be formed) but to supply

  sources of notae. They have more in common with notarial abbreviations

  and punctuation than they do with the phonemic graphemes of an intelli-

 

‹ Prev