The Gap

Home > Other > The Gap > Page 38
The Gap Page 38

by Thomas Suddendorf


  266oldest indication of violence: Keeley, 1996; Thorpe, 2003.

  266northern Iraq (Shanidar 3): Churchill et al., 2009.

  266The jaw of a 30,000-year-old: Ramirez Rozzi et al., 2009.

  266paradigm shift: Gibbons, 2011.

  266African data also suggest interbreeding: Hammer et al., 2011.

  Chapter 12: Quo Vadis?

  269eight times the biomass: Smil, 2002.

  270the first script was: Schmandt-Besserat, 1992.

  270(Footnote 2) Those who continued: Brody, 2000.

  271historical novel Creation: Vidal, 1981.

  271(Footnote 3) Stanislas Dehaene: Dehaene, 2009.

  272“The library connects us: Sagan, 1980, p. 282.

  273Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: See http://royalsocietypublishing.org/journals/.

  274Systema Naturae: Linnaeus, 1758.

  275to make models and predictions: e.g., Borjeson et al., 2006.

  275recognized as global problems: Diamond, 2005; Singer, 2002.

  278Darwinian man: Gilbert & Sullivan, 2010.

  278critical to complement the genetic data: Varki et al., 1998.

  278narrow down the search space: Suddendorf & Butler, 2013.

  280genetic changes in human populations: Milot et al., 2011.

  280brain sizes decreased: McAuliffe, 2010.

  280brain size and IQ: McDaniel, 2005.

  280“I do not know with what: Johnson, 2005.

  281Countless civilizations have collapsed: Diamond, 2005.

  281we have domesticated ourselves: e.g., Hare et al., 2012.

  281overall decline in violence: Pinker, 2011a.

  282Some evidence suggests that brain size: Jantz, 2001.

  283Plan it for the apes: See the website of the Great Apes Survival Partnership (GRASP): http://www.un-grasp.org.

  REFERENCES

  Adams, D. (1979). The hitchhiker’s guide to the galaxy. London: Pan.

  Addis, D. R., et al. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377.

  Addis, D. R., et al. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychological Science, 19, 33–41.

  Alemseged, Z., et al. (2006). A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature, 443, 296–301.

  Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In P. Mellars & C. Stringer (Eds.). The human revolution: behavioral and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 455–513). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

  Alloway, T. P., et al. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716.

  Ambrose, S. H. (2001). Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science, 291, 1748–1753.

  Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2011). Which primates recognize themselves in mirrors. PLOS Biology, 9, 1–3.

  Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953–970.

  Arensburg, B., et al. (1990). A reappraisal of the anatomical basis for speech in middle paleolithic hominids. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 83, 137–146.

  Asfaw, B., et al. (1999). Australopithecus garhi: A new species of early hominid from Ethiopia. Science, 284, 629–635.

  Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320.

  Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.

  Azevedo, F. A. C., et al. (2009). Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 513, 532–541.

  Baars, B. J. (2005). Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience, Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 150, pp. 45–53).

  Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.

  Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.

  Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3 1/2-month-old and 4 1/2-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–664.

  Balter, M. (2010). Did working memory spark creative culture? Science, 328, 160–163.

  Balter, M. (2012a). Did Neandertals truly bury their dead? Science, 337, 1443–1444.

  Balter, M. (2012b). Early dates for artistic Europeans. Science, 336, 1086–1087.

  Balter, M. (2012c). Ice age tools hint at 40,000 years of bushman culture. Science, 337, 512.

  Balter, M. (2012d). ‘Killjoys’ challenge claims of clever animals. Science, 335, 1036–1037.

  Bar, M. (2011). Predictions in the brain: Using our past to generate a future. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Bard, K. A. (1994). Evolutionary roots of intuitive parenting: Maternal competence in chimpanzees. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 19–28.

  Bard, K. A. (2003). Are humans the only primates that cry? Scientific American, June 16.

  Bard, K. A., et al. (2006). Self-awareness in human and chimpanzee infants: What is measured and what is meant by the mark and mirror test. Infancy, 9, 191–219.

  Barkow, J. H., et al. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Bradford/MIT Press.

  Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.

  Baron-Cohen, S., et al. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.

  Bartal, I. B.-A., et al. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science, 334, 1427–1430.

  Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Bateson, P. (1991). Assessment of pain in animals. Animal Behaviour, 42, 827–839.

  Batki, A., et al. (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 223–229.

  Bauer, P. (2007). Remembering the times of our lives: memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.

  Beirne, P. (1994). The law is an ass: Reading E. P. Evans’ “The medieval prosecution and capital punishment of animals.” Society and Animals, 2, 27–46.

  Bekoff, M., & Pierce, J. (2009). Wild justice: The moral lives of animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Bennett, M. R., et al. (2009). Early hominin foot morphology based on 1.5-million-year-old footprints from Ileret, Kenya. Science, 323, 1197–1201.

  Bentley-Condit, V. K., & Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour, 147, 185–221.

  Berger, L. R., et al. (2010). Australopithecus sediba: A new species of Homo-like Australopith from South Africa. Science, 328, 195–204.

  Berna, F., et al. (2012). Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, E1215–E1220.

  Berwick, R. C., et al. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 89–98.

  Bird, C. D., & Emery, N. J. (2009). Rooks use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating worm. Current Biology, 19, 1410–1414.

  Bischof, N. (1985). Das Rätzel Ödipus [The Oedipus riddle]. Munich: Piper.

  Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation [On the phylogeny of human motivation]. In L. H. Eckens
berger & E. D. Lantermann (Eds.), Emotion und Reflexivität (pp. 3–47). Vienna: Urban & Schwarzenberg.

  Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71, 727–731.

  Boesch, C. (1990). Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 54, 86–99.

  Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530–532.

  Boesch, C. (1994). Chimpanzees-red colobus monkeys: A predator-prey system. Animal Behaviour, 47, 1135–1148.

  Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984). Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25, 160–170.

  Bogin, B. (1999). Evolutionary perspective on human growth. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 109–153.

  Boring, E. G. (1923). Intelligence as the test tests it. New Republic, 35, 35–37.

  Borjeson, L., et al. (2006). Scenario types and techniques: Towards a user’s guide. Futures, 38, 723–739.

  Bouzouggar, A., et al. (2007). 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 9964–9969.

  Bowles, S. (2009). Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science, 324, 1293–1298.

  Boyd, R., et al. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 10918–10925.

  Boysen, S. T., & Hallberg, K. I. (2000). Primate numerical competence: Contributions toward understanding nonhuman cognition. Cognitive Science, 24, 423–444.

  Bramble, D. M., & Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature, 432, 345–352.

  Brauer, J., et al. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175–181.

  Braun, D. R., et al. (2010). Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 10002–10007.

  Breuer, T., et al. (2005). First observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLOS Biology, 3, 2041–2043.

  Brody, H. (2000). The other side of Eden: Hunters, farmers and the shaping of the world. London: Faber & Faber.

  Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297–299.

  Brosnan, S. F., et al. (2005). Tolerance for inequity may increase with social closeness in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 272, 253–258.

  Brown, F., et al. (1985). Early Homo erectus skeleton from West Lake Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 316, 788–792.

  Brown, K. S., et al. (2009). Fire as an engineering tool of early modern humans. Science, 325, 859–862.

  Brown, P. (2012). LB1 and LB6 Homo floresiensis are not modern human (Homo sapiens) cretins. Journal of Human Evolution, 62, 201–224.

  Brown, P., et al. (2004). A new small-bodied hominin from the Late Pleistocene of Flores, Indonesia. Nature, 431, 1055–1061.

  Browning, R. (1896). The poetical works. London: Smith, Elder & Co.

  Brüne, M., & Brüne-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437–455.

  Brunet, M., et al. (1996). Australopithecus bahrelghazali, a new species of early hominid from Koro Toro region, Chad. Comptes Rendus De L Academie Des Sciences Serie Ii Fascicule a—Sciences De La Terre Et Des Planetes, 322, 907–913.

  Brunet, M., et al. (2002). A new hominid from the Upper Miocene of Chad, Central Africa. Nature, 418, 145–151.

  Burkart, J. M., et al. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19762–19766.

  Busby, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2005). Recalling yesterday and predicting tomorrow. Cognitive Development, 20, 362–372.

  Busby Grant, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2009). Preschoolers begin to differentiate the times of events from throughout the lifespan. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 746–762.

  Busby Grant, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2010). Young children’s ability to distinguish past and future changes in physical and mental states. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 853–870.

  Busby Grant, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2011). Production of temporal terms by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 87–95.

  Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary Psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

  Butler, D. L., et al. (2012). Mirror, mirror on the wall, how does my brain recognize my image at all? PLOS One, 7, e31452.

  Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–721.

  Byrne, R. W., & Tanner, J. (2006). Gestural imitation by a gorilla. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 6, 215–231.

  Cabana, T., et al. (1993). Prenatal and postnatal growth and allometry of stature, head circumference, and brain weight in Quebec children. American Journal of Human Biology, 5, 93–99.

  Call, J. (2001a). Chimpanzee social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 388–393.

  Call, J. (2001b). Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 159–171.

  Call, J. (2004). Inferences about the location of food in the great apes (Pan pansicus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 232–241.

  Call, J. (2006). Inferences by exclusion in the great apes: The effect of age and species. Animal Cognition, 9, 393–403.

  Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 192–206.

  Call, J., et al. (1998). Chimpanzee gaze following in an object choice task. Animal Cognition, 1, 89–99.

  Call, J., et al. (2004). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7, 488–498.

  Calvin, W. H. (1982). Did throwing stones shape hominid brain evolution? Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 115–124.

  Cann, R. L., et al. (1987). Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature, 325, 31–36.

  Cannell, A. (2002). Throwing behaviour and the mass distribution of geological hand samples, hand grenades and Olduvian manuports. Journal of Archaeological Science, 29, 335–339.

  Carbonell, E., et al. (2008). The first hominin of Europe. Nature, 452, 465–469.

  Carroll, L. (1871). Through the looking glass. London: Macmillan.

  Casey, B. J., et al. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 14998–15003.

  Caspari, R., & Lee, S. H. (2004). Older age becomes common late in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 10895–10900.

  Cavalieri, P., & Singer, P. (1995). The great ape project: Equality beyond humanity. New York: St. Martin’s Griffin.

  Chahl, J. S., et al. (2004). Landing strategies in honeybees and applications to uninhabited airborne vehicles. The International Journal of Robotics Research, 23, 101–110.

  Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The Chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.

  Cheke, L. G., & Clayton, N. S. (2012). Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) overcome their current de
sires to anticipate two distinct future needs and plan for them appropriately. Biology Letters, 8, 171–175.

  Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 28, 362–376.

  Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Churchill, S. E., & Rhodes, J. A. (2009). The evolution of the human capacity for “killing at a distance.” Dordrecht: Springer.

  Churchill, S. E., et al. (2009). Shanidar 3 Neandertal rib puncture wound and paleolithic weaponry. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 163–178.

  Ciochon, R. L. (1996). Dated co-occurrence of Homo erectus and Gigantopithecus from Tham Khuyen Cave, Vietnam. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 3016–3020.

  Clayton, N. S., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature, 395, 272–278.

  Clayton, N. S., et al. (2001). Elements of episodic-like memory in animals. Philosophical Transactions: Royal Society of London, B, 356, 1483–1491.

  Clayton, N. S., et al. (2007). Social cognition by food-caching corvids: The western scrub-jay as a natural psychologist. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 362, 507–522.

  Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377–395.

  Collier-Baker, E., & Suddendorf, T. (2006). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and 2-year-old children (Homo Sapiens) understand double invisible displacement? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 89–97.

  Collier-Baker, E., et al. (2004). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) understand invisible displacement? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 421–433.

  Collier-Baker, E., et al. (2005). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) understand single invisible displacement? Animal Cognition, 9, 55–61.

  Coppens, Y. (1994). East side story: The origin of humankind. Scientific American, 270, 62–69.

  Corballis, M. C. (2003). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. New York: Princeton University Press.

  Corballis, M. C. (2007a). Recursion, language, and starlings. Cognitive Science, 31, 697–704.

 

‹ Prev