Gertrude Stein (Critical Lives)

Home > Other > Gertrude Stein (Critical Lives) > Page 3
Gertrude Stein (Critical Lives) Page 3

by Lucy Daniel


  Gertrude, meanwhile, had plunged into the era of new sciences which branched out from evolution theory. Her major courses were in psychology, zoology and botany; among her tutors were William James, Josiah Royce, Hugo Münsterberg and George Santayana, all engaged in explaining the ‘American mental quality’,10 as well as Charles B. Davenport, who was to become America’s leading eugenicist. During this period Stein’s lifelong interest in defining character solidified. That Stein trained as a psychologist and a zoologist in the laboratories at Harvard is of paramount importance to all her subsequent literary experiment.11 Her scientific persona had a direct effect on her literary compositions. Mina Loy recognized this in her poetical tribute to Stein in 1924:

  Curie

  of the laboratory

  of vocabulary

  she crushed

  the tonnage

  of consciousness

  congealed to phrases

  to extract

  a radium of the word12

  In point of fact Stein was useless as a chemist. But her work in experimental psychology, neuropsychology and zoology, and then in the mapping of the brain that was going on at Johns Hopkins, gave her a unique relation, as a creative writer, to the science of her day.

  When Stein first entered the Harvard Psychology Laboratory in 1893, the science itself was in its infancy. For the orphaned Stein, to whom so many doors were open, so many possibilities on the horizon, who saw herself springing into a new world in which the laws of the old were no longer necessarily applicable (she shared the common millenarian note of her era), psychology was a new science with which to classify that world, a system to grasp onto. And one of its fathers, that unconventional champion of liberated thinking, William James, was her teacher. His Principles of Psychology, one of the founding texts of the new science, had been published three years before Gertrude enrolled at Radcliffe. In her sophomore year Stein studied under James’s protégé, Hugo Münsterberg, who wrote to her at the end of the year that she had been his ‘ideal student’.13 Münsterberg believed that every psychological occurrence had its basis in a physiological one. So satisfactory was Stein’s first year’s work of experimental psychology with him in the area of acoustics that she was invited the following year by James himself to take part in his seminars, and carry out experiments supervised by him in person.

  Here is Stein’s (perhaps apocryphal) note to Professor James at the top of an end of year exam paper, which she abandoned unwritten: ‘Dear Professor James, … I am so sorry but really I do not feel a bit like an examination paper in philosophy to-day’; and his supposed riposte: ‘Dear Miss Stein, I understand perfectly how you feel I often feel like that myself’ — after which, she claimed, he gave her top marks, and she still got to go to the opera.14 The anecdote distils a certain dilettantish attitude to her scientific career, but also a desire to present herself as a favourite of the great man. In her third year at Radcliffe, in 1896, under James’s suggestion and guidance, Stein embarked on a series of experiments into automatism with her classmate Leon Solomons.

  ‘Automatic writing’ was at the time a favourite tool of mediums and spiritualists who claimed it provided a connection with the spirit world. William James himself was on famously equivocal and open terms with this spiritualist view of the mind’s powers. But psychology officially considered automatic writing a door instead to the unconscious regions of human thought. Its use was particularly common in the treatment of hysteria, as hysterics were supposed to be more susceptible to allowing that buried ‘second personality’ to express itself.

  Technically what Stein and Solomons produced was distracted writing.15 They would read a novel and distractedly scribble sentences at the same time; or one of them would read a piece of writing while the other read something aloud to them, and they eventually achieved a state of mind that allowed them to carry out both acts simultaneously. The aim was to distinguish how the sort of ordinary, distracted acts that everyone performs every day, without paying attention to them, shaded into the so-called ‘second consciousness’ of the hysteric.16 In other words, to show that what was attributed to that ‘second personality’ could be done by normal people in a distracted state, thereby disproving the theory of a second personality. By achieving a sort of dissociation from their own acts by distraction, Stein and Solomons summarized that hysteria was ‘a disease of the attention’. This work was eventually published as ‘Normal Motor Automatism’. In their joint publication Stein and Solomons write that they are ‘both … perfectly normal — or perfectly ordinary’;17 by attributing normality to themselves they become assimilated within the objectivity, neutrality and universality claimed by contemporary science.18 The question of what constitutes the ‘normal’ would be one which lingered in Stein’s work ever after.

  In 1934, when Stein’s literary work had gained both popularity and notoriety, it was the subject of a critique by the psychologist B. F. Skinner, published prominently in The Atlantic Monthly, entitled ‘Has Gertrude Stein a Secret?’ Skinner claimed that, using the techniques of automatic writing, in Tender Buttons (published in 1914) Stein had invented a second personality for her narrator, one without a past or much intellect. Many readers followed Skinner into using automatic writing as an answer to the puzzle of Stein’s more challenging techniques, her ‘unintelligible’ sentences. Couching their claims in fairly hostile if not outrightly misogynist language, critics not only claimed that Stein was a practitioner of automatic writing, but suggested that because Stein’s poetry resembled its dissociated effects, it amounted to a proof of her own hysterical tendencies, her own ‘degenerate’ nature. It was a censorious view that linked the breaking of literary rules with moral laxity, a view of words themselves as morally diseased. It has been one of the most lingering myths surrounding Stein’s work, though it has often been refuted.

  Stein herself disputed the idea that any of her work was produced by automatic writing, and noted that she didn’t even think what she and Solomons had produced in the laboratory could properly be classed as automatic writing — she didn’t think it was possible under laboratory conditions. She said that ‘writing for the normal person is too complicated an activity to be indulged in automatically.’19 She did not believe, in fact, that unless one was hypnotized, it was possible to produce writing that did not in some way ‘make sense’. How she described the result of the experiments was that ‘A phrase would seem to get into the head and keep repeating itself at every opportunity … The stuff written was grammatical, and the words and phrases fitted together all right, but there was not much connected thought.’20 Stein and Solomons had been looking for writing with nothing, no consciousness behind it — but there was, she later contended, no such thing. Speaking specifically about Tender Buttons, she told an interviewer: ‘I made innumerable efforts to make words write without sense and found it impossible. Any human being putting down words had to make sense of them’ (my emphasis).21 That attempt at transference of agency to the words themselves, rather than the consciousness behind them, was an important admission. The removal of meaning from words had been part of the experiment of Tender Buttons; it just hadn’t worked. She would continue to insist on exploring the freedom of words from meanings and antecedents: ‘I like the feeling of words doing as they want to do and as they have to do’, she later wrote.22

  Stein and Solomons had, after all (with a certain amount of scientific naivety) used themselves as subjects. The lines that Stein produced in the experiment with Solomons showed, as they reported, ‘a marked tendency to repetition’ — later a characteristically ‘Steinian’ device — and some of them seem to sound preemptive echoes of her later style: ‘When he could not be the longest and thus to be, and thus to be the strongest’.23

  Stein, with skull and microscope, late 1890s.

  What Skinner disregarded in his attempt to discredit Stein’s writing by noting the similarities of the finished product to automatic writing, and linking them to the outpourings of hysteria, was
Stein’s awareness of her own effects. She was bound to deny it, but there is little doubt that, although in later years she did not actively practice automatic writing, Stein was intrigued by the idea, and she was obsessed by the question of how writing is linked to consciousness. She became, with her background in psychology and neuropsychology, a powerful thinker and theorist on the relation of writing to the work of the brain, and her ‘experimental’ writing — perhaps the most experimental work produced by any writer of the twentieth century — depended on recognition of aspects of how writing works that are normally left implicit or unnoticed.24 As Steven Meyer argues in his detailed study of the relation of Stein’s scientific career to her creative writing, in doing so she not only absorbed scientific ideas into her writing, nor merely wrote in a spirit of scientific enquiry, nor simply used science as a metaphor, but made her creative writing itself into a kind of experimental science.25 In science, the failure of one experiment could lead the practitioner on to further discoveries; so Stein included her failures in her work. In the midst of her most grammatically and verbally ‘experimental’ period, she wrote ‘I see I have a trained eye I do microscopic work’.26 Although she later tried to shake off the experimental model (in Everybody’s Autobiography), this was partly to do with her reluctance to be called an ‘experimental writer’, a tag which seemed to suggest inferiority, a secondary status in the pantheon of literary greatness. She preferred to see herself as an inventor or, most of all, a genius.

  Her experience of ‘automatic writing’ primarily instilled in her the new and radical perspective of seeing writing as an artefact, a product rather than a process, and gave her an idea of the words on the page as objects in themselves which need not necessarily be related to one another, a Saussurean realization of the randomness of the linguistic sign (although, of course, independently arrived at), a slippage of meaning in words as signifiers which could bring them into new relations with one another. Her attempts at removing meaning from words were both disconcerting and, eventually, liberating. In automatic writing, it was the physical process of the formation of words and word groupings by an independently moving pencil that supplied her with a new vision of the power of words as nothing more or less than words, writing as pure behaviour.27 (One of Stein’s hobbies was reading people’s handwriting. In doing so, again, she was not looking at the meaning of the words on the page, but regarding them as objects which provided indirect clues to character.) It was also the first of her enquiries into ‘How Writing is Written’, which would become a pervasive theme of her own work. In the 1930s she would lecture on words and their relation to consciousness, the mind of words.

  Later she would counsel a younger writer that: ‘creation must take place between the pen and the paper’, rather than in planned thought before composition.28 (She had a lifelong disgust for typewriters.) She never denied the similarity in appearance between her work and so-called automatic writing; what she denied was the presence of the automatic element within herself.

  In a theme written at the time of these experiments, Stein describes the physical experience of automatic writing as a form of torture: a girl is strapped into a sort of machine for writing,

  her finger imprisoned in a steel machine and her arm thrust immovably into a big glass tube … Strange fancies begin to crowd upon her, she feels that the silent pen is writing on and on forever. Her record is there she cannot escape it … her imprisoned misery.29

  This is a striking extrapolation of the mechanical apparatus and processes of the laboratory into the psyche, via the action of writing. Writing itself becomes a source of distress. Just as the hydrocephalic, hysterical subject of her earlier theme ‘An Annex Girl’ had collapsed under the weight of books, here the girl is shut in the prison of her own hysterical composition. She was, in these themes, exploring not only ‘The Value of College Education for Women’ (the title of a speech she gave to a Baltimore Women’s Group in 1898), but the value of contemporary scientific procedure: ‘Before long this vehement individual is requested to make herself a perfect blank while someone practices on her as an automaton.’ Stein was a ‘vehement individual’; she did not easily succumb to the idea of herself as an ‘automaton’. She would repudiate Skinner’s claims four decades later for much the same reason.

  The natural outcome for Stein of this adverse image of the hysterical woman writer was to abscond herself from the possibility of being included in it. As Stein put it in The Autobiography, she ‘never had subconscious reactions’.30 She made this doubly reassuring by couching it in a story about William James, in which she made her half-fictional James imply that whatever she did (however abnormal), it was normal: an all-satisfying, all-justifying claim. Paradoxically she wanted all her abnormalities to be wrapped up and cosseted in a vision of the normal that still allowed, indulged her individuality. Her interest in character was partly an egotistical urge to understand her own character, her own composition, at this time. ‘I was tremendously occupied with finding out what was inside myself to make what I was.’31 This included her relative masculinity or femininity. In her notebooks she described herself as a ‘masculine type’.

  The feminist debate of the period was tied in with the pseudoscientific definition of the very meaning of masculinity and femininity. Experimental psychology was the testing ground for Stein’s ideas on this subject; later they would gain a shaky philosophical prop in Weininger’s Sex and Character. Weininger’s belief in women’s particular susceptibility to hypnotism, and propensity to ‘psychic automatism’, an example of feminine lack of will, may have influenced Stein’s claims that she was not a useful participant in automatic experiments (Weininger ‘despised’ the idea of a subconscious).32

  Taking a much needed change of surroundings in the summer break after these experiments in 1896, Gertrude joined Leo in Antwerp; it was an archetypal literary-aesthetic tour for well-connected Americans in Europe. They toured Holland and Germany, Gertrude an eager apprentice to all her brother’s theorizing about art, then fleetingly stopped in Paris, and then London for a month, which did not strike Gertrude favourably. When she returned to Radcliffe for her final year she would further develop her experiments in automatic writing, this time on her own, and this time she would combine them with her growing interest in character types.

  Working this time without Solomons, the plan was to discover the susceptibility of various character types to automatic behaviour, not necessarily writing; to see how different types differed in their habits of attention. The subject’s arm would be placed on a planchette hung from the ceiling; in their hand they held a pencil. Stein would talk or read to them, and when they seemed distracted by her words she would surreptitiously guide their hand, so that they would draw a shape on the paper. (The image of the student Stein manipulating her subjects’ hands, while boldly attempting to peer into their minds, seems to foretell the mature Stein’s domineering intellectual stance as the chronicler of writing’s relation to consciousness.) She classified the results according to type. Most of the subjects were what she called the ‘New England’ type — which she classified as morbidly self-conscious. Type I were nervous, with strong powers of concentration. Type II were blonde, pale and emotional. They had poor powers of concentration, were suggestible, likely to produce automatic movement, and verging on the hysterical.33 These ideas were published in the cutting edge journal The Psychological Review under the title ‘Cultivated Motor Automatism’: Stein’s first printed work, since her earlier work with Solomons had been written up by him. Groping towards a scientific system for the basis of identity with a melange of ideas taken from morphology, physiognomy and typology, Stein described her work as a study of ‘the nervous conditions of men and girls at college’.34 She was already mapping out for herself a career as a doctor; she had a particular interest in nervous diseases in women (often related in the period’s medical literature to sexual or reproductive disorders). Later her work would be cited in the hysteria section of Havelock Ell
is’s groundbreaking work of sexology, Studies in the Psychology of Sex.

  Already this schematic arrangement of character traits suggests the system that would engulf Stein in her urge to catalogue every kind of person who ever could be living. Her scientist’s eye was trained not to see an external governing force behind emotions and personality, but to see personality forged through habit and repetition. In The Making of Americans (written between 1903 and 1911), Stein offers a range of answers to the problem of what character is, or of what exactly it is constituted; the notion of ‘kinds’, then ‘bottom nature’, then ‘being’; but its basis, even in her pursuit of a new literary language with which to cope with such ideas, is in experimental psychology, in finding physical phenomena to represent psychological ones, as Münsterberg had taught her.

  How quickly and how slowly, how completely, how gradually, how intermittently, how noisily, how silently, how happily, how drearily, how difficultly, how gaily, how complicatedly, how simply, how joyously, how boisterously, how despondingly, how fragmentarily, how delicately, how roughly, how excitedly, how energetically, how persistently, how repeatedly, how repeatingly, how drily, how startlingly, how funnily, how certainly, how hesitatingly, anything is coming out of that one, what is being in each one and how anything comes into that one and comes out of that one makes of each one one meaning something and feeling, telling, thinking, being certain and being living.35

  Those persistent ‘hows’ both pose myriad questions and suggest that they have been or will be in some way answered. Stein poses as the scientific, egalitarian observer of character formation through habit, as if she has all the answers, but in fact she equivocates, or experiments, over and over again. Her adoption of an empirical, scientific viewpoint in the novel led her to devote several notebooks to laying out schemes and diagrams of types and their connections to one another. She scurrilously compiled judgemental sketches of the various types as visible among her friends.

 

‹ Prev