The Power of Time Perception
Page 24
66.Buetti, S. & Lleras, A. Perceiving control over aversive and fearful events can alter how we experience those events: an investigation of time perception in spider-fearful individuals. Front. Psychol. 3, 337 (2012).
67.Droit-Volet, S., Fayolle, S. L. & Gil, S. Emotion and time perception: effects of film-induced mood. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5, 33 (2011).
68.ANDERSON, M. J., REIS-COSTA, K. & MISANIN, J. R. Effects of September 11th Terrorism Stress on Estiamted Duration. Percept. Mot. Skills 104, 799–802 (2007).
69.LANGER, J., WAPNER, S. & WERNER, H. The effect of danger upon the experience of time. Am. J. Psychol. 74, 94–97 (1961).
70.Watts, F. N. & Sharrock, R. Fear and time estimation. Percept. Mot. Skills 59, 597–598 (1984).
71.Tipples, J. Negative emotionality influences the effects of emotion on time perception. Emotion 8, 127–131 (2008).
72.Gil, S., Niedenthal, P. M. & Droit-Volet, S. Anger and time perception in children. Emotion 7, 219–225 (2007).
73.Mostofsky, E., Penner, E. A. & Mittleman, M. A. Outbursts of anger as a trigger of acute cardiovascular events: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Heart J. (2014).
74.Izard, C. E. & Ackerman, B. P. in Handbook of emotions xvi, 720 (2000).
75.Freed, P. J. & Mann, J. J. Sadness and Loss : Toward a Neurobiopsychosocial Model. 28–34 (2007).
76.Mezey, a. G. & Cohen, S. I. the Effect of Depressive Illness on Time Judgment and Time Experience. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 24, 269–270 (1961).
77.Sneed, R. S. & Cohen, S. A prospective study of volunteerism and hypertension risk in older adults. Psychol. Aging 28, 578–586 (2013).
78.Konrath, S., Fuhrel-Forbis, A., Lou, A. & Brown, S. Motives for volunteering are associated with mortality risk in older adults. Heal. Psychol. 31, 87–96 (2012).
79.Bar-Haim, Y., Kerem, A., Lamy, D. & Zakay, D. When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cogn. Emot. 24, 255–263 (2010).
80.Wittmann, M., Vollmer, T., Schweiger, C. & Hiddemann, W. The relation between the experience of time and psychological distress in patients with hematological malignancies. Palliat. Support. Care 4, 357–363 (2006).
81.Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R. & Baumeister, R. F. Social exclusion and the deconstructed state: time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 409–423 (2003).
82.Bargh, J. A., Chen, M. & Burrows, L. Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 230–244 (1996).
83.Effron, D. A., Niedenthal, P. M., Gil, S. & Droit-Volet, S. Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion 6, 1–9 (2006).
84.MacLean, K. A., Johnson, M. W. & Griffiths, R. R. Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 1453–61 (2011).
85.Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D. & Aaker, J. Awe expands people’s perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1130–6 (2012).
86.Charles Darwin. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. (D. Appleton & Company, 1872).
87.Endukuru, C. K., Maruthy, K. N. & Deepthi, T. S. A Study of Critical Flickering Fusion Frequency Rate in Media Players. Volume: 2, 499–502 (2015).
88.Jackson, L. A. et al. Information technology use and creativity: Findings from the Children and Technology Project. Comput. Human Behav. 28, 370–376 (2012).
89.Kovess-Masfety, V. et al. Is time spent playing video games associated with mental health, cognitive and social skills in young children? Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. (2016). doi:10.1007/s00127-016-1179-6
90.Psychology Today. Self-test at
91.Geen, R. G. Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extroverts: Effects on arousal and performance.
92.Conte, J. M., Schwenneker, H. H., Dew, A. F. & Romano, D. M. Incremental Validity of Time Urgency and Other Type A Subcomponents in Predicting Behavioral and Health Criteria. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 31, 1727–1748 (2001).
93.Locsin, R. C. Time experience of selected institutionalized adult clients. Clin. Nurs. Res. 2, 452–463 (1993).
94.Esposito, M. J. et al. Prospective time estimation over a night without sleep. Biological Rhythm Research 38, 443–450 (2007).
95.W. Christopher Winter. Sleep type predicts day and night batting averages of Major League Baseball players. American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) (2011). at
96.Wahlstrom, K. Changing Times: Findings from the First Longitudinal Study of Later High School Start Times. (2002).
97.Wieth, M. B. & Zacks, R. T. Time of day effects on problem solving: When the non-optimal is optimal. Think. Reason. 17, 387–401 (2011).
98.O’Brien, E. H., Anastasio, P. A. & Bushman, B. J. Time crawls when you’re not having fun: Feeling entitled makes dull tasks drag on. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 1287–1296 (2011).
99.Kakizaki, M. et al. Personality and body mass index: a cross-sectional analysis from the Miyagi Cohort Study. J. Psychosom. Res. 64, 71–80 (2008).
100.Website. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Online Test. Self-Test at
101.Murray, J. B. Marijuana’s effects on human cognitive functions, psychomotor functions, and personality. J. Gen. Psychol. 113, 23–55 (1986).
102.Lieving, L. M., Lane, S. D., Cherek, D. R. & Tcheremissine, O. V. Effects of marijuana on temporal discriminations in humans. Behav. Pharmacol. 17, 173–83 (2006).
103.Harles T., T. Marijuana Intoxication: Common Experiences. Nature 226, 701–704 (1970).
104.Perez-Reyes, M., Burstein, S. H., White, W. R., McDonald, S. A. & Hicks, R. E. Antagonism of marihuana effects by indomethacin in humans. Life Sci. 48, 507–15 (1991).
105.Sewell, R. A. et al. Acute effects of THC on time perception in frequent and infrequent cannabis users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 226, 401–13 (2013).
106.Matell, M. S., King, G. R. & Meck, W. H. Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 150–6 (2004).
107.Bruce, M., Scott, N., Lader, M. & Marks, V. The psychopharmacological and electrophysiological effects of single doses of caffeine in healthy human subjects. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 22, (1986).
108.Rolland, B. et al. Pharmacology of hallucinations: several mechanisms for one single symptom? Biomed Res. Int. 2014, 307106 (2014).
109.Matell, M. S., Bateson, M. & Meck, W. H. Single-trials analyses demonstrate that increases in clock speed contribute to the methamphetamine-induced horizontal shifts in peak-interval timing functions. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 188, 201–212 (2006).
110.MacDonald, C. J. & Meck, W. H. Differential effects of clozapine and haloperidol on interval timing in the supraseconds range. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 182, 232–244 (2005).
111.Brozek, E. S. and J. Flicker Fusion Frequency: Background and Applications. Physiol. Rev. 32, (1952).
112.Hindmarch, I., Alford, C., Barwell, F. & Kerr, J. S. Measuring the side effects of psychotropics: the behavioural toxicity of antidepressants. J. Psychopharmacol. 6, 198–203 (1992).
113.Smith, A. P. Caffeine at work. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 20, 441–5 (2005).
114.Smith, A., Brice, C., Nash, J., Rich, N. & Nutt, D. J. Caffeine and central noradrenaline: effects on mood, cognitive performance, eye movements and cardiovascular function. J. Psychopharmacol. 17, 283–92 (2003).
115.Arushanyan, E. B., Baida, O. A., Mastyagin, S. S., Popova, A. P. & Shikina, I. B. Influence of Caffeine on the Subjective Perception of Time by Healthy Subjects in Dependence on Various Factors. Hum. Physiol. 29, 433–436 (2003).
116.Davranche, K. & Audiffren, M. Effects of a low dose of transdermal nicotine on information processing. Nicotine Tob. Res. 4
, 275–285 (2002).
117.Turner, D. C. et al. Cognitive enhancing effects of modafinil in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 165, 260–9 (2003).
118.Müller, U. et al. Effects of modafinil on non-verbal cognition, task enjoyment and creative thinking in healthy volunteers. Neuropharmacology 64, 490–5 (2013).
119.Müller, U., Steffenhagen, N., Regenthal, R. & Bublak, P. Effects of modafinil on working memory processes in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 177, 161–9 (2004).
120.Baldwin, R. L. et al. Effect of methylphenidate on time perception in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 12, 57–64 (2004).
121.Allman, M. J. & Meck, W. H. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain 135, 656–77 (2012).
122.Coull, J. T., Cheng, R.-K. & Meck, W. H. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 3–25 (2011).
123.Yang, B. et al. Time perception deficit in children with ADHD. Brain Res. 1170, 90–96 (2007).
124.Curran, S. & Wattis, J. Critical flicker fusion threshold: A potentially useful measure for the early detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 15, 103–112 (2000).
125.Bentivoglio, A. R. et al. Analysis of blink rate patterns in normal subjects. Mov. Disord. 12, 1028–34 (1997).
126.Karson, C. N. Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain 106 (Pt 3), 643–53 (1983).
127.Kowal, M. A., Colzato, L. S. & Hommel, B. Decreased Spontaneous Eye Blink Rates in Chronic Cannabis Users: Evidence for Striatal Cannabinoid-Dopamine Interactions. PLoS One 6, e26662 (2011).
128.Friedman, W. J. & Wilkins, A. J. Scale effects in memory for the time of events. Mem. Cognit. 13, 168–175 (1985).
129.Brown, N. R., Rips, L. J. & Shevell, S. K. The subjective dates of natural events in very-long-term memory. Cognitive Psychology 17, 139–177 (1985).
130.Whitten, W. B. & Leonard, J. M. Directed search through autobiographical memory. Mem. Cognit. 9, 566–579 (1981).
131.Le Poidevin, R. The Experience and Perception of Time. Stanford Encycl. Philos. (2000). at
132.Ornstein, R. E. On the Experience of Time. (1969). at
133.Wackermann, J. Determinants of filled/empty optical illusion: differential effects of patterning. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars). 72, 89–94 (2012).
134.Wearden, J. H., Norton, R., Martin, S. & Montford-Bebb, O. Internal clock processes and the filled-duration illusion. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 716–29 (2007).
135.Avni-Babad, D. & Ritov, I. Routine and the perception of time. Journal of experimental psychology. General 132, (2003).
136.Zakay, D. Attention allocation policy influences prospective timing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5, 114–118 (1998).
137.Block, R. a & Zakay, D. Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 4, 184–197 (1997).
138.Wearden, J. H. The wrong tree : time perception and time experience in the elderly. 134–156 (2005).
139.D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O. & Van Der Linden, M. Frequency, characteristics and functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 96–103 (2011).
140.Botzung, A., Denkova, E. & Manning, L. Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel. Brain Cogn. 66, 202–212 (2008).
141.Bilgin, B. & LeBoeuf, R. A. Looming Losses in Future Time Perception. Journal of Marketing Research 47, 520–530 (2010).
142.Margolies, S. O. & Crawford, L. E. Event valence and spatial metaphors of time. Cogn. Emot. 22, 1401–1414 (2008).
143.Scheier, M. F. et al. Dispositional optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: the beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1024–1040 (1989).
144.Patrick Fanning. Visualization for Change. (New Harbinger Publications, 1994).
145.Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D. & Armor, D. A. Harnessing the imagination. Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. The American psychologist 53, 429–439 (1998).
146.Konečni, V. J. & Ebbesen, E. B. Distortions of Estimates of Numerousness and Waiting Time. J. Soc. Psychol. 100, 45–50 (1976).
147.Buehler, R., Griffin, D. & Ross, M. Exploring the ‘planning fallacy’: Why people underestimate their task completion times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 366–381 (1994).
148.Buehler, R. & Griffin, D. Planning, personality, and prediction: The role of future focus in optimistic time predictions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 92, 80–90 (2003).
149.Buehler, R., Griffin, D., Lam, K. C. H. & Deslauriers, J. Perspectives on prediction: Does third-person imagery improve task completion estimates? Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 117, 138–149 (2012).
150.Gallant, R., Fidler, T. & Dawson, K. A. Subjective time estimation and age. Percept. Mot. Skills 72, 1275–1280 (1991).
151.Joubert, C. E. Structured time and subjective acceleration of time. Percept. Mot. Skills 59, 335–336 (1984).
152.Vanneste, S., Pouthas, V. & Wearden, J. H. Temporal Control of Rhythmic Performance: A Comparison Between Young and Old Adults. Experimental Aging Research 27, 83–102 (2001).
153.Mangan, P. & P. Bolinskey. Underestimation of time during normal aging: The result of the slowing of a dopaminergic regulated internal clock? in Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience (1997).
154.Wittmann, M. & Lehnhoff, S. Age effects in perception of time. Psychol. Rep. 97, 921–935 (2005).
155.Friedman, W. J. & Janssen, S. M. J. Aging and the speed of time. Acta Psychol. (Amst). 134, 130–41 (2010).
156.Rammsayer, T. H. Effects of body core temperature and brain dopamine activity on timing processes in humans. Biol. Psychol. 46, 169–192 (1997).
157.Curran, S., Hindmarch, I., Wattis, J. P. & Shillingford, C. Critical flicker fusion in normal elderly subjects; a cross-sectional community study. Curr. Psychol. 9, 25–34 (1990).
158.Lachenmayr, B. J. et al. The different effects of aging on normal sensitivity in flicker and light-sense perimetry. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 35, 2741–2748 (1994).
159.Carstensen, L. L. Evidence for a Life-Span Theory of Socioemotional Selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science 4, 151–156 (1995).
160.Jiga-Boy, G. M., Clark, A. E. & Semin, G. R. So much to do and so little time. Effort and perceived temporal distance. Psychol. Sci. a J. Am. Psychol. Soc. / APS 21, 1811–1817 (2010).
161.Tang, Y.-Y. et al. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 17152–6 (2007).
162.Davidson, R. J. et al. Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosom. Med. 65, 564–570 (2003).
163.Li, P., Legault, J. & Litcofsky, K. a. Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex. 1–24 (2014). doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001
164.Van Boven, L. & Ashworth, L. Looking forward, looking back: anticipation is more evocative than retrospection. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 289–300 (2007).
165.Nawijn, J., Marchand, M. A., Veenhoven, R. & Vingerhoets, A. J. Vacationers Happier, but Most not Happier After a Holiday. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 5, 35–47 (2010).
166.Tali Sharot. The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain. (Pantheon, 2011). at
About the Author
Jean Paul Zogby, author of The Power of Time Perception and The Time Miracle is a writer, researcher, music composer, and real estate development expert.
His current passion is to help people understand why time speeds up and how to make the most of it. With the last 6 years spent researching Time Perception in the fields of Neuroscience and Cognitiv
e Psychology, he is passionate about sharing what science has to say about our experience of time.
In his free time, Jean Paul composes soundtrack music for film and also publishes research related to the formation of planetary systems in the field of Astrophysics.
A husband and father of two lovely daughters and a son, he resides in Dubai where he is the CEO on a multi-billion construction project.
For more details, visit the Official Website at http://www.jpzogby.com where you can subscribe to my mailing list and receive the free guide to a Healthy Brain Diet.
If you enjoyed this book or received value from it in any way, I would appreciate if you would be kind enough to leave a review for this book on Amazon at the link below.
https://www.amazon.com/product-reviews/0995734771/
It would be greatly appreciated!