Book Read Free

Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Page 6

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  15

  St - Yves , M. & Tanguay , M. ( 2009 ). The psychology of interrogation: A quest for a

  confession or a quest for the truth? In M. St - Yves & M. Tanguay (Eds), The

  psychology of criminal investigations: The search for the truth (pp. 9 – 40 ). Toronto :

  Thomson Carswell .

  St - Yves , M. , Tanguay , M. & Cr é pault , D. ( 2004 ). La psychologie de la relation: cinq

  r è gles de base . In M. St - Yves & J. Landry (Eds.), Psychologie des entrevues d ’ enqu ê te:

  de la recherche à la pratique (pp. 135 – 153 ). Cowansville, Qu é bec : Yvon Blais .

  Ury , W. ( 1991 ). Getting past no: Negotiating with diffi cult people . New York : Bantam

  Books .

  Wald ,

  M. ,

  Ayres ,

  R. ,

  Hess ,

  D. W.

  ,

  Schantz ,

  M. & Whitebread ,

  C. H.

  (

  1967 ).

  Interrogations in New Haven. The impacts of Miranda . Yale Law Journal , 76 ,

  1519 – 1648 .

  Walsh , D. P. J. ( 1982 ). Arrest and interrogation: Northern Ireland 1981 . Journal of

  Law and Society , 9 , 37 – 62 .

  Walters , S. B. ( 1995 ). Principles of kinesic interview and interrogation . New York :

  CRC .

  Williamson , T. M. ( 1990 ). Strategic changes in police interrogation: an examination of

  police and suspect behaviour in the Metropolitan Police in order to determine the

  effects of new legislation, technology and organizational policies. Unpublished PhD

  thesis, University of Kent.

  Williamson , T. M. ( 1993 ). From interrogation to investigative interviewing: Strategic

  trends in police questioning

  .

  Journal of Community and Social Psychology ,

  3 ,

  89 – 99 .

  Williamson , T. M. ( 2006 ). Investigative interviewing and human rights in the war on

  terrorism . T. Williamson (Ed.), Investigative interviewing: Rights, research, regula-

  tion (pp. 3 – 22 ). Cullompton : Willan Publishing .

  Zulawski , D. & Wicklander , D. ( 1992 ). Practical aspects of interview and interrogation .

  New York : CRC .

  Cases

  R v Oickle [ 2000 ] 2 R.C.S.3 .

  R v Rothman [ 1981 ] 1 R.C.S. 640 .

  Chapter Two

  A Typology of Denial Strategies by

  Suspects in Criminal Investigations

  Stephen Moston

  Department of Psychology, James Cook University

  and

  Geoffrey M. Stephenson

  Department of Psychology, London South Bank University

  In this chapter we propose a typology of denials made by suspects in police

  interviews. This is an important forensic topic, fi rst, because police offi cers

  need to consider in advance of an interview the likely strategies that might be

  adopted by those who intend to deceive them concerning their involvement

  in a criminal offence. Can offi cers nullify such attempts before the suspect

  commits to such a strategy? How should they respond if such a denial is not

  averted? Secondly, and perhaps more important still, is the ability to distinguish

  between the denials of those intent on deceit and those who are genuinely

  innocent of any involvement. Should all denials be treated with scepticism? Or

  should all denials be taken at face value pending discovery of contradictory

  evidence? Should denials be welcomed on the grounds that evidence will sub-

  sequently be used to prove the intention to deceive? Or should denials best

  be circumvented by adroit questioning, in the hope that a confession will

  subsequently be elicited?

  We shall see that experts are not at one on these issues (e.g. Gudjonsson,

  2006 ) and, moreover, that there is a dearth of evidence about the form that

  denials take generally. Before examining the questions further, we shall describe

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

  Edited by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine and Tom Williamson

  © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

  18

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  briefl y what is known about the opposite stance taken by suspects: confession

  to the crime in connection with which they are being interviewed.

  Confessions in the p olice s tation

  Since the introduction of the Police and Criminal Evidence Act 1984 (PACE)

  in the United Kingdom there have been several major observational studies,

  with sample sizes in excess of 500 cases, on police interviewing styles and

  responses to accusations of criminal wrongdoing by suspects in criminal inqui-

  ries by police offi cers (e.g., Moston, Stephenson & Williamson, 1992 ; Baldwin,

  1993 ; Moston & Stephenson, 1993 ; Bucke & Brown, 1997 ; Phillips & Brown,

  1998 ; Bucke, Street & Brown, 2000 ). In addition, there have been several

  smaller - scale studies focusing on specifi c populations (e.g., juveniles; Evans,

  1993 ) or focused research issues (e.g., the impact of psychological vulnerabili-

  ties on confessions; Pearse, Gudjonsson, Clare & Rutter, 1998 ).

  In each of these studies the percentage of suspects who confessed during

  interviews has been reasonably consistent, ranging from a low of 55% (Moston

  et al. , 1992 ; Phillips & Brown, 1998 ; Bucke et al. , 2000 ) to a high of 62%

  (Baldwin,

  1993 ), with other studies falling within this narrow range (e.g.

  Bucke

  & Brown,

  1997 : 58%; Pearse

  et al. ,

  1998 : also 58%; Moston

  &

  Stephenson, 1993 : 59%; Evans, 1993 : also 59%).

  Surprisingly, there have been few comparable studies in other countries. In

  the USA there have only been two comparable attempts to quantify confession

  rates. Cassell & Hayman (1996) reported that 42% of suspects made confes-

  sions or admissions, while Leo (1996) suggested the confession/admission

  rate reached 64%. In Australia, Dixon & Travis (2007) provide data on two

  separate samples of cases, with one sample giving a confession/admission rate

  of 76%, while in the second sample the rate was lower, at 46%.

  International comparisons are fraught with diffi culties, as the sample sizes

  are often relatively small (e.g., 87 cases in Dixon & Travis ’ s second sample)

  and different sampling techniques are used (e.g., Cassell and Haymann ’ s study

  used a non - random sample). We must also recognize that police offi cers in

  different jurisdictions may be more or less scrupulous about whom they accuse

  of a particular crime. A similar variation may also occur between different forces

  in the UK. Indeed, there have been well - documented cases of remarkably high

  clear - up rates in some forces, suggesting that crimes ‘ taken into consideration ’

  (TICs), to which suspects have obligingly confessed, have boosted the percent-

  age of successful prosecutions (Gill, 1987 ).

  Denials in the p olice s tation

  Most suspects who do not confess, in one way or another deny their culpabil-

  ity. Others may remain silent or otherwise evade answering directly, although

  A Typology of Denial Strategies by Suspects in Criminal Investigations 19

  in the UK the numbers so doing may have decreased since a revised caution
r />   was introduced warning of the dangers should their silence subsequently be

  used to indicate guilty concealment. Existing data on the percentage of sus-

  pects who deny are harder to interpret, with some studies either failing to

  mention the denial rate altogether, or collapsing data (including denials,

  evasion and use of the right to silence) into a single category, perhaps better

  described as a ‘ non - confession ’ rate. Such problems notwithstanding, studies

  suggest that in the UK between 35% (Moston & Stephenson, 1993 ) and 45%

  (Phillips & Brown, 1998 ) of suspects deny involvement or do not confess.

  Data from pre - PACE studies in the UK show a similar pattern, with Softley,

  Brown, Forde, Mair & Moxon (1980) reporting a denial rate of 35%, and

  Irving (1980) a rate of 42%, suggesting that changes in police questioning

  practices following the introduction of PACE and the mandatory tape

  -

  recording of interviews had little impact on this particular metric of

  interviewing effectiveness.

  Why do s uspects c onfess?

  Moston et al. (1992) analysed over 1,000 cases in the UK in which suspects

  were interviewed by police offi cers. In addition to collecting detailed records

  of the cases through a questionnaire attached to custody records, they also

  analysed over 500 taped records of interviews. Their research found clear links

  between three case characteristics (strength of evidence, offence severity and

  legal advice) and the outcome of interviews. It was suggested that police

  interviewing techniques played a relatively minor role in infl uencing confes-

  sions. There were two reasons for this conclusion.

  First, most admissions were freely volunteered at the outset of interviews

  and those suspects who denied an accusation at the outset of an interview

  typically maintained this denial throughout, even in the face of seemingly

  incontrovertible proof of their guilt. A similar pattern of results was found by

  Baldwin (1993) , with only nine suspects from a sample of 600 showing a

  response shift from denial to admission during the course of questioning.

  The second and perhaps most surprising observation was that police inter-

  viewing skills were not greatly in evidence. Only a limited range of questioning

  techniques was employed, and interviewers often appeared to be more nervous

  than the suspects they were questioning. Many offi cers concluded interviews

  at the fi rst sign of resistance from suspects, whilst others doggedly continued

  with repetitive, stereotyped questioning or assertions.

  Such fi ndings run contrary to the popular myth concerning police interviews

  with suspects. It is probably the case that most people believe that a typical

  interrogation begins with an initial denial from the suspect, but that by skilled

  questioning the interviewer reveals the inconsistencies in that person ’ s state-

  ment or its confl ict with other evidence. When confronted with the high-

  lighted contradictions, the suspect recants their denial and makes a full

  20

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  confession. Such a dazzling sequence does occur in rare instances, but it is

  probably not that common.

  Directions in r esearch on p olice i nterviewing

  Much psychological research on the process of police interviews with suspects

  has focused on how the inappropriate use of interviewing tactics can result in

  false confessions from innocent suspects (e.g., Kassin & Gudjonsson, 2004 ;

  Kassin, 2005 ). The impact of the same interviewing tactics on the decision -

  making of guilty suspects has largely been neglected. One reason for this stands

  out above all others: a number of those who deny their involvement in criminal

  activity are likely to be telling the truth, with the consequence that routine

  employment of deception, ‘ minimization ’ , threats, false promises and other

  devious practices may result in an unacceptable number of erroneous convic-

  tions; this is especially problematic given the weight that has normally been

  accorded to confession evidence. Interviewing (with the aim of securing a

  confession) can all too easily be used as a shortcut to a search for independent

  evidence. Police offi cers are not often in the position of having undeniable

  proof of guilt before suspects are interviewed. If they have such evidence, then

  a confession is not essential but merely convenient. In fact, by the same token,

  interviews will sometimes be used by police in order to eliminate from further

  inquiry those suspects against whom there is little evidence or about whom

  there is, rightly or wrongly, a presumption of innocence.

  The current emphasis on instructions to police offi cers in the UK is to

  interview to establish ‘ the truth ’ and to make no prior assumptions regarding

  guilt or innocence. In principle, suspects are to be treated as witnesses, albeit

  potentially, but not necessarily, as witnesses to their own guilt. The truth or

  falsity of denials, whatever form these denials may take, will be evaluated

  according to their plausibility when considered in relation to other evidence.

  If rigorously followed, this ethical, ideal route to justice will ensure a minimum

  of false convictions. However, police time is not unlimited, and investigations

  to prove the truth of a confession obtained in response to skilled questioning

  will undoubtedly be more speedily concluded than those made without assis-

  tance from the suspect. Research into the veracity or evidence value of different

  categories of denial may be of considerable value to the effectiveness and

  overall integrity of police interviewing.

  The emphasis on avoiding false confessions has such clear importance for

  civil liberties that the current emphasis on the need to avoid prior assumptions

  of guilt needs little justifi cation. The complexity of establishing the effective-

  ness of specifi c interviewing tactics on suspects, be they guilty or innocent,

  undoubtedly exerts a powerful infl uence on research design. Farrington ( 1981 :

  100) offered the following reasons why it was almost impossible to identify

  the effects of varying interrogation styles on suspect behaviour because of

  variations in other variables.

  A Typology of Denial Strategies by Suspects in Criminal Investigations 21

  The relationship between style of interrogation and likelihood of confessing

  could be investigated in real life in a correlational study. Such a study could show

  that one style was associated with a signifi cantly higher likelihood of confessing

  than another. However, it would be impossible to attribute this difference to the

  style variable in such a study, because of all the uncontrolled variables. For

  example, one style might be preferred by certain police offi cers who were more

  effective in inducing confessions for some reason unconnected with style; or one

  style might be used with certain suspects who might be more likely to confess

  than others (again for reasons unconnected with style). Only an experiment can

  isolate the variable of style and demonstrate unambiguously that it infl uences

  confessing.

  Experiments have indeed established that ‘ confessing ’ can be in
creased by

  interviewing tactics, although, unfortunately, at the risk of increasing the false

  confession rate. For example, subsequent experimental research on police

  interviewing has shown that certain well - used tactics, such as minimization

  (Kassin & McNall, 1991 ), can increase the number of people who will falsely

  confess to a range of minor transgressions they have not committed. While

  such research is interesting, it often neglects to consider the potential impact

  of such tactics on guilty suspects. Further, an unwritten assumption seems to

  be that guilty suspects are relatively homogeneous and that a given tactic will

  be equally effective no matter what variations in offence (e.g., property offences

  vs. person offences), suspect (e.g., presence or absence of prior criminal

  history), interviewer (e.g., male vs. female) or evidence (e.g., eyewitness vs.

  fi ngerprint evidence) might exist. This is an important oversight as such vari-

  ables can have a powerful infl uence on the attitudes and assumptions of inter-

  viewing offi cers. For example, Stephenson & Moston (1993) found that prior

  criminal history and strength of evidence predicted whether or not suspects

  were assumed to be guilty prior to questioning in the police station. However,

  an experimental study by Weber (2007) involving retired police offi cers found

  that only strength of evidence, and not criminal history, predicted presump-

  tions of guilt.

  It follows that differing assumptions about guilt or innocence may have a

  bearing on the choice of interviewing strategy. Experimental research by

  Kassin, Goldstein & Savitsky (2003) , using mock interrogators and suspects,

  has shown that the presumption of guilt has a strong infl uence on the types

  of questions interrogators choose, with presumed guilt prompting more

  aggressive interrogations, which constrained the responses of the suspects.

  Similarly, Weber (2007) found that presumption of guilt was linked to the

  selection of confession - oriented tactics in mock interview plans developed by

  (retired) police offi cers.

  A similar argument can be advanced for understanding a suspect ’ s strategy

  during an interview. Variations in offence, suspect, interviewer or evidence

  might also be expected to infl uence how a suspect responds to an accusation,

 

‹ Prev