Book Read Free

Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Page 8

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  cates them. This is always a useful strategy for suspects, who can then decide

  whether or not to make any admissions depending on the strength of evidence

  against them. Second, it is an attempt to justify a criminal act as a consequence

  of alcohol/drugs, not criminal intent, that is, the offence was almost an

  ‘ accident ’ .

  Denial of m otivation. Some suspects challenge on the grounds that the

  offence details do not fi t because they are, ‘ just not my style ’ , as in, ‘ I ’ ve

  done karate. If I ’ d hit that person they ’ d never get up ’ . Similar types of appeal

  might also be heard in burglary cases: ‘ You know me. I only pinch what I can

  carry. ’

  A common theme here is that the suspect is appealing to the interviewer

  for understanding, asking to be believed. Crucially, in such instances, suspects

  do not say that they did not commit the action; rather, that it simply does not

  fi t their usual pattern, it is not in their character, dubious though their char-

  acter admittedly is. This may well be most commonly used by those with

  considerable previous experience of police questioning. The suspect cleverly

  avoids directly responding to the accusation with an outright denial, giving

  the interviewer the chance to draw the right conclusion.

  A variation on this strategy involves suspects challenging the accusation on

  the grounds that they would not have committed the offence because it ‘ simply

  wasn ’ t worth it ’ . For example, the amount of money that could have been

  taken would have been very small, or in the case of goods, they would not

  have been worth taking. On some occasions the suspect may make reference

  to the possible punitive consequences that could have arisen: ‘ It wouldn ’ t be

  worth going to prison for the sake of a few quid. ’ There is a further variation

  on this strategy, outlined by Inbau et al. (2001) in their description of the

  ‘ specifi c denial ’ strategy of a suspect who says, ‘ I didn ’ t shoot her with a Colt.

  357. ’

  28

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Active d enials

  Denial of o ffence. Seemingly a common defence in cases in which children

  have alleged that they had been sexually abused has been the claim that it was

  ‘ All a fi gment of their imagination ’ . In fact, this challenge is common in many

  sexual offences, even those in which adults are the accusers. It is clearly an

  attempt to undermine the credibility of the witness.

  Interviewers tend to counter this defence by asking something like

  ‘ So you ’ re saying this girl made all this up? ’ This may force the suspect to

  attack the credibility of the witness, either directly and personally or in terms

  of broad generalizations, such as ‘ Kids always make up stuff like that, don ’ t

  they? ’

  The other common response to the interviewer ’ s question is ‘ No, I ’ m not

  saying that ’ , thus inviting an accusation of being self - contradictory, but prob-

  ably designed to play for time in order to keep their defensive options open.

  The attempt to force the suspect ’ s hand by asking something along the lines

  of, ‘ Then what are you saying? Is she a liar or isn ’ t she? ’ is unlikely to secure

  an admission and may play into the suspect ’ s hands, if, as does happen, the

  interviewer asks the suspect to explain why that person might bear a grudge

  or have a reason to lie. Although the suspect ’ s explanation for the accuser ’ s

  lack of credibility may be at a general level, as with the example ‘ Kids always

  make up stuff like that ’ , a specifi c explanation based on past exchanges between

  the suspect and accuser are more likely to impress. The latter is likely to be

  perceived as more credible.

  Denial of i nterpretation. Some suspects suggest that the police have mis-

  interpreted an innocent action. Although on many occasions this could well

  be the case, this strategy is rarely used in a way that conveys credibility.

  Suspects sometimes appear unsure of the motives for their own actions and

  fail to offer a coherent explanation of events. In the following example,

  following an assault, a person has been arrested while running from the police:

  Interviewer : Why were you running?

  Suspect :

  Well, everyone else started running and I thought they were

  running for a train or something, so I ran as well, then one

  of your lot grabbed me.

  One obvious problem here is the ‘ or something ’ that is included in the

  explanation. The suspect may well be hedging, that is keeping their options

  open in order to avoid presenting a story that might confl ict with the account

  of any others who may also have been arrested. On occasions, interviewers

  may fi nd it almost impossible not to contain their disbelief when this strategy

  is used, as is illustrated in the following example.

  A Typology of Denial Strategies by Suspects in Criminal Investigations 29

  Interviewer : But why do you think that he [the witness] would want to

  accuse you of a serious offence?

  Suspect : I

  don

  ’ t know.

  Interviewer : The way you say it that you was walking along the street, next

  thing you know you was grabbed by a bloke who ’ s got a knife

  and you haven ’ t a clue what ’ s going on.

  Suspect :

  I just really don ’ t know. I don ’ t know. I was baffl ed at the time,

  like all I was worried about was getting away from the knife,

  you know what I mean?

  Interviewer : Seems very strange. … but you see, it just leaves that big gaping

  doubt. Why would a man want to accuse you of something

  you hadn ’ t done …

  Suspect : I

  don

  ’ t know.

  Interviewer :

  … when you ’ ve never seen him before in your life?

  Suspect: I

  don

  ’ t know. Perhaps he mistook me for someone. I don ’ t

  know.

  This type of denial probably reduces the credibility of the speaker. The

  suspect ’ s statement contains so many hedges and ambiguities that it app-

  ears that his recall of the incident is especially poor. In practice, this style

  of denial will probably be interpreted as a lie, but certainly not necessarily

  deservedly.

  Denial of c ausation. Some suspects try to challenge facts, such as being

  found in possession of stolen goods, by suggesting that they did not know

  that the items were stolen and that they had borrowed them from a friend

  (thereby shifting the blame onto others). The suspect is not challenging the

  evidence itself, since that is likely to be incontestable; instead, they are chal-

  lenging the assumptions that can be drawn from it, and in particular that they

  are guilty of an offence.

  The purpose of this challenge is to divert attention from the suspect by

  drawing attention to another potential suspect, who may or may not be speci-

  fi ed. One aspect of this challenge is that the suspect will probably be reluctant

  to directly accuse another person; often they are merely trying to raise the

  possibility that another person w
as involved. This challenge often loses cred-

  ibility when the interviewer directly asks the suspect if the second person is

  the person responsible for the offence, as in the following example.

  Suspect : I

  didn

  ’ t steal the tapes; I just borrowed them from Andy

  ’ s

  house.

  Interviewer : But

  I

  ’ ve just told you they were stolen, so are you saying that

  Andy stole them?

  Suspect :

  Well, no, I ’ m not saying that.

  30

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Another classic scenario here would be ‘ I didn ’ t steal it. I bought it from a

  man in the pub ’ . This strategy is low in credibility simply because it is intended

  to be vague (the man in the pub never has a name!) and thus diffi cult to verify.

  This denial strategy is often used as a direct consequence of police interviewing

  practices which place the suspect in a situation involving two undesirable

  options: accept the evidence or implicate a friend. If the latter course is chosen,

  the interviewer may have problems in deciding which of the suspects is truthful

  and which is deceptive.

  A variation on this strategy is to invoke the hand of God:. ‘ I don ’ t know

  how they got there ’ is a good illustration of this in relation to stolen goods.

  Here, the suspect suggests that the evidence against them (stolen property

  found in their possession) had mysteriously appeared and that they were even

  more surprised to fi nd it than the police. There is no attempt to attribute any

  cause to this minor miracle, the police will not be accused of fabricating evi-

  dence (although some interviewers may feel this is being implied), nor will

  another person be implicated. It is as if the hand of God somehow intervened

  in the matter and placed the items there.

  The key aspect of this denial strategy is the lack of any speculation about

  how the items came to be in the suspect ’ s possession because, again, the guilty

  suspect will be reluctant to be tied down to a single explanation of events

  which might be discredited.

  Presumed g uilty

  Offi cers in many countries tend to work on the premise that a good outcome of

  an interrogation is a confession (Stephenson & Moston, 1993 ; Weber, 2007 ),

  interviewing competence often being defi ned by the numbers of confessions

  elicited (Blair, 2005 ). This approach sets the scene for the possibility of false

  confessions, in that offi cers may adopt questioning techniques that coerce the

  suspect into retracting their earlier statement. Although, traditionally, offi cers

  are likely to interpret the retraction as justifying coercive techniques, UK judges

  have more recently taken a very different view. Confession evidence alone is

  unlikely to be persuasive, and confessions made under any kind of duress run a

  real risk of not being admitted in evidence (Williamson, 2006 ).

  The current emphasis on the avoidance of false confessions refl ects the great

  emphasis on the role played by police questioning techniques, specifi cally the

  psychological ploys adopted to manipulate the suspect ’ s decision - making. For

  example, police questioning techniques are thought to be largely responsible

  for eliciting both coerced - compliant and coerced - internalized false confessions

  (see Kassin & Gudjonsson, 2004 ). This view has not been helped by the

  limited literature on police interviews with suspects (e.g., Inbau et al. , 2001 ),

  which tends to concentrate on how to overcome denials and elicit confessions.

  This emphasis remains prevalent in the USA and no doubt many other juris-

  dictions (Buckley, 2006 ).

  A Typology of Denial Strategies by Suspects in Criminal Investigations 31

  Psychological research on the statements of child victims of sexual abuse

  has led to the development of techniques for analysing the accuracy of state-

  ments, such as criteria - based content analysis (for a review, see Vrij, 2005 ).

  The underlying assumption of these techniques is that the statements of a

  truthful child and a deceptive child are inherently different. Similar assump-

  tions underlie police training in the detection of lie signs (e.g., Walkley, 1987 ).

  Lie signs are specifi c statements that are believed to be indicative of guilt. For

  example, a verbal lie sign would be a statement such as ‘ I hope my mother

  drops dead if I ’ m lying ’ or ‘ I swear on my kid ’ s life ’ . Such statements, it is

  argued (e.g., Inbau et al. , 2001 ), are typically used by guilty suspects who

  overstate their innocence. Certain phrases may suggest deception because of

  their inherent lack of credibility, but they could also come from suspects with

  poor memories, or who were possibly trying to protect another person. It will

  also become apparent that certain forms of denial are sometimes a direct result

  of particular police questioning techniques.

  Implicit in the notion of overstated innocence is the idea that guilty suspects

  protest too strongly. This assumes that an innocent person will protest their

  innocence at an optimal level. Such ideas lack any empirical evidence and are

  essentially ‘ words of wisdom ’ passed on by experienced investigators. Given

  that police offi cers are notoriously poor at detecting deceit, such statements

  can only be treated as spurious suggestions that are probably best avoided.

  There are, however, a number of encouraging research developments on

  the detection of deception that encourage the expectation that greater under-

  standing of the behaviour of suspects in response to accusations, and in par-

  ticular variation in denial strategies, may contribute to the scientifi c analysis of

  deception (e.g. Frank, Yarbrough

  & Ekman,

  2006 ). We shall conclude,

  however, with a consideration of how in practice police offi cers respond to

  denials by suspects.

  Handling d enials

  One of the most obvious stumbling blocks for police offi cers when questioning

  a suspect is handling their challenges or denials. For police offi cers, it can be

  quite disconcerting to fi nd that suspects do not immediately accept the seem-

  ingly obvious, namely, that they must be guilty. Many suspects challenge the

  evidence or deny the allegation, no matter how incontrovertible or incontest-

  able it seems to the investigating offi cer. Police interviewers often appear to

  attribute denials to the stupidity or stubbornness of the suspect. A denial is

  met with a degree of disbelief refl ecting the view that the suspect was just too

  stupid to accept that the evidence was clear - cut. On other occasions a denial

  is dismissed on the grounds that ‘ We just didn ’ t have enough evidence ’ . That

  is, the interviewer would undoubtedly have obtained an admission if only they

  had gathered that bit more evidence. Finally, some interviewers may feel that

  they talked the suspect into a denial. The interview may have gone badly, with

  32

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  the interviewer failing to sound convincing or giving a false impression of the

  (strong) evidence against the suspect.
/>   This chapter has shown that there is a wide range of strategies that suspects

  use when denying an allegation. Some strategies involve explicit denials, whilst

  others are more evasive, with some suspects willing to provide detailed answers

  to questions, whilst others will confi ne their replies to short, dismissive state-

  ments. One obvious problem that can arise during questioning is that when

  probing for additional verifying information, interviewers give the impression

  that they do not believe anything that the suspect has said. This may forestall

  any subsequent responses. If this occurs, the interviewer might make the

  unfortunate mistake of assuming that the person must have been lying.

  Although it would be wrong to say that some forms of challenge by suspects

  are more likely to involve deception than others, it may be that interviewers

  perceive certain forms of reply as less credible than others. As a future hypoth-

  esis, it may be that certain types of denials, used in particular circumstances,

  are relatively unusual and thus predictive of credibility. Denial strategy might

  form one component for a form of statement validity analysis for adult suspects

  and contribute to the refi nement of systems of interviewing analysis (e.g. Frank

  et al. , 2006 ).

  References

  Baldwin , J. ( 1993 ). Police interview techniques: Establishing truth or proof? British

  Journal of Criminology , 33 , 325 – 352 .

  Blair , J. P. ( 2005 ). What do we know about interrogation in the United States? Journal

  of Police and Criminal Psychology , 20 ( 2 ), 44 – 57 .

  Bucke , T . & Brown , D. ( 1997 ). In police custody: Police powers and suspects ’ rights under

  the revised PACE codes of practice. A research and statistics directorate report .

  London : Home Offi ce .

  Bucke , T. , Street , R. & Brown , D. ( 2000 ). The right of silence: The impact of the

  Criminal Justice and Public Order Act 1994. A research, development and statistics

  directorate report . London : Home Offi ce .

  Buckley J. P.

  (

  2006 ).

  The Reid Technique of interviewing and interrogation

  . In

  T. Williamson (Ed.),

  Investigative interviewing: Research, rights, regulation .

  Cullompton : Willan Publishing .

  Cassell , P. G. & Hayman , B. S. ( 1996 ). Police interrogation in the 1990s: An empirical

 

‹ Prev