Book Read Free

Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing: Current Developments and Future Directions

Page 17

by Ray Bull, Tim Valentine, Dr Tom Williamson


  transcript shows an admission, and then later admissions that should have been

  denials ( ibid .: 181).

  A total of 30 terrorist cases from the CTC at New Scotland Yard qualifi ed

  for this study. Each case was studied in relation to the case details and the

  custody record checked for medical observations or indications of psychologi-

  cal vulnerability. The interview tapes were cross - referenced with the relevant

  transcripts and then divided into interview sessions for analysis. An interview

  session was defi ned as any period of continuous interviewing (i.e., with no

  breaks in excess of 60 minutes). In previous studies cases were often analysed

  according to the individual time of each tape, but this failed to capture the

  full extent and practicalities involved in the police interview. For example, it

  is not unusual to allow consultations with legal advisers to take place, or short

  adjournments for exhibits and refreshments. A total of 187 interview sessions

  were analysed. Thus with an appreciation of all relevant allegations and some

  knowledge of the individual suspect ’ s detention experience, the author was in

  a position to begin to analyse each case.

  Interview t actics and q uestionnaire d istribution

  The author has analysed many hundreds of police interviews and has developed

  a robust methodology for understanding and interpreting what takes place

  within this particular interaction. Essentially, the nature and type of tactics

  employed by the police can be captured within a specially designed coding

  frame predicated on a three dimensional typology: Delivery – Maximization

  – Manipulation (Pearse, 1997 ; see also Pearse, 2006 . This extends the meth-

  odology to help understand the tactics employed at the Abu Ghraib and

  Guantanamo Bay detention centres).

  Delivery is very much an overlapping category which is present throughout

  the process and is designed to capture

  ‘ what ’ type of questions are asked

  (closed, leading, open etc.); ‘ how ’ they are put (hushed, lowered tones, or

  The Investigation of Terrorist Offences in the United Kingdom

  79

  harsh and aggressive) as well as any relevant contextual features (i.e. ‘ where ’

  the questioning takes place – the custodial environment). It captures all the

  contextual features. Maximization is ‘ a hard - sell ’ technique in which the inter-

  rogator tries to scare and intimidate the suspect into confessing by making

  claims (false or otherwise) about evidence and exaggerating the seriousness

  and/or magnitude of the charges (Pearse, 2006 : 76). This includes any tactic

  designed to increase a suspect ’ s internal anxiety. Finally, manipulation relates

  to the more subtle and insidious form of questioning that attacks a suspect ’ s

  perception of the crime or the consequences of his or her actions and will

  include manipulating details, or signifi cant others, fl attery, making unrealistic

  promises, as well as attacking a suspect ’ s self - esteem or stature.

  A total of 50 (71%) questionnaires were returned by CT offi cers out of 70

  randomly distributed within the branch. The respondents were offered the

  traditional range of formats to answer questions, including ‘ yes ’ or ‘ no ’ , or

  ‘ not selected ’ . Unfortunately, on occasions some respondents failed to make

  any selection. This has been recorded for statistical purposes as ‘ not selected ’

  and will inevitably impact on the total responses per question and statistical

  fi ndings. Many months of delay were experienced throughout the period of

  this research in accessing materials or personnel whenever a major case or

  investigation was under way. In a small number of cases (fi ve) an isolated

  audio - tape could not be found, but in each instance the relevant transcript was

  available. To date three custody records cannot be located.

  Findings and c ommentary

  One of the fi rst issues to arise from the face - to - face interviews with serving

  CT offi cers was the predicament that they were placed in when called upon

  to make an early arrest. The offi cers were asked:

  There is a suggestion that the strategic decision to arrests suspects prematurely

  (often to save life) impacts to the detriment of the detention and interviewing

  process. For example, by immediately imposing time pressure and without

  recourse to available and suffi cient evidence, e.g. surveillance, forensic, computer

  access, fi nancial and international inquiries. To what extent do you agree with

  this?

  Forty - one offi cers (82%) ‘ agreed ’ or ‘ strongly agreed ’ with this proposition

  (Table 5.1 ).

  In criminal arrests the ethos underpinning PACE was to provide police with

  suffi cient powers to gather evidence prior to arrest and so reduce the period

  of detention. By contrast, there is often a need to arrest terrorist suspects early

  in order to protect the public and save lives. Such action may be based on

  sensitive intelligence and often before there has been time to gather suffi cient

  evidence that would allow the prosecution to consider there was a likelihood

  80

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Table 5.1: Impact of early arrest on interview process

  Frequency

  Percentage

  Cumulative

  N = 50

  Strongly disagree

  5

  10%

  10%

  Disagree

  4

  8%

  18%

  Agree

  23

  46%

  64%

  Strongly agree

  18

  36%

  100%

  of conviction (see the letter from Assistant Commissioner Andy Hayman to

  the Home Secretary, Rt. Hon. Charles Clarke, http://www.homeoffi ce.gov.

  uk/Terrorism/Terrorismandthelaw).

  In this study such ‘ public safety ’ arrests precipitated a series of sterile and

  non - productive rounds of antecedent interviews, often preceded by vague

  disclosure notices, in turn prompting ‘ no comment ’ advice from resolute legal

  advisers: a cyclical impasse generating palpable levels of frustration and effec-

  tively undermining any opportunity for the offi cers to manage events, a situ-

  ation best captured by one of the CT offi cers who, when placed in this

  dilemma, made the plea: ‘ Unless we get evidence soon, we may as well start

  telling jokes ’ (data obtained during debriefi ng sessions).

  It was noticeable that over a third of all interview sessions (37%) took place

  within the fi rst two days of detention, with a majority dominated by anteced-

  ent inquiries. Gareth Peirce, solicitor, in her evidence to the Home Affairs

  Select Committee (Terrorism Detention Powers, Fourth Report of Session

  2005 – 06, Vol. II, p. ev 13), was of the opinion that 90% of interviews follow

  this pattern. The same activity also limits the opportunity to utilize the fi ndings

  of relevant psychological studies that have identifi ed the importance of the

  suspect ’ s perception of events and in particular the nature, or their perception

  of the nature, of the evidence against them. According to the offi cers, the

  frequency of p
ublic safety arrests often places them in an untenable position

  and a negative mantra has evolved: why bother interviewing, [we have nothing

  to put to them and] the suspect is not going to say anything? One offi cer

  described the situation as similar to ‘ being stacked in a holding pattern over

  Heathrow – waiting for information, waiting for evidence to continue ’ .

  In psychological terms, if we adopt the offi cers ’ perspective, they are already

  under pressure – required to interview international terrorist suspects in a very

  accountable environment – and they feel further exposed in public safety cases

  by the absence of credible evidence to allow a meaningful dialogue to take

  place. This heightened state of dependency and a negative frame of mind serve

  as a fertile combination to create an enduring self fulfi lling prophecy.

  Table 5.2 provides details of the extent of the use of various tactics by police

  offi cers during the 187 interview sessions. The most immediate fi nding is the

  limited use of many of the categories. ‘ Use of evidence ’ (95%) was the most

  popular tactic and covered the production of exhibits, photographs and other

  The Investigation of Terrorist Offences in the United Kingdom

  81

  Table 5.2: Extent of use of police interviewing tactics

  Use

  Never Used

  Infrequent

  Average

  Frequent

  Very

  N = 187

  Use

  Use

  Use

  Frequent

  Tactics

  Use

  Open question

  0

  3

  75

  104

  5

  (1.5%)

  (40%)

  (56%)

  (2.5%)

  Closed question

  4

  54

  105

  21

  3

  (2%)

  (29%)

  (56%)

  (11%)

  (2%)

  Leading question

  84

  73

  27

  2

  1

  (45%)

  (39%)

  (14.5%)

  (1%)

  (0.5%)

  Repeat question

  159

  20

  8

  0

  0

  (85%)

  (11%)

  (4%)

  Multiple question

  165

  14

  7

  1

  0

  (88%)

  (7.5%)

  (4%)

  (.5%)

  Multiple offi cers

  175

  7

  5

  0

  0

  (94%)

  (3.5%)

  (2.5%)

  Challenges

  56

  42

  63

  19

  7

  (30%)

  (22.5%)

  (34%)

  (10%)

  (3.5%)

  Improper tactics

  184

  3

  0

  0

  0

  (98%)

  (2%)

  Manipulative

  150

  17

  19

  1

  0

  tactics

  (80.5%)

  (9%)

  (10%)

  (.5%)

  Minimization

  170

  12

  5

  0

  0

  (91%)

  (6.5%)

  (2.5%)

  Maximization

  123

  18

  39

  7

  0

  (66%)

  (9.5%)

  (21%)

  (3.5%)

  Use of evidence

  10

  23

  51

  63

  40

  (5%)

  (12%)

  (27%)

  (34%)

  (22%)

  materials, but it tended to occur in a rather sterile and perfunctory atmos-

  phere. The limited use of ‘ challenges ’ is indicative of the polite and non -

  confrontational nature of these interactions, even though the use of silence by

  important suspects was prevalent. Given previous research from 1999 it was

  also surprising to fi nd such limited use of maximization tactics (emphasizing

  the serious nature of offence), minimization (reducing suspect ’ s perception of

  consequences) or manipulation (use of themes, fl attery) (Pearse & Gudjonsson,

  1999 ).

  Such data are responsible for the summary that, in this study, interviews

  with terrorist suspects can best be described as ‘ Polite, non - threatening and

  often non - productive ’ .

  Little use was made of the special warning procedure (sections 34 – 37 of

  the Criminal Justice and Public Order Act 1994; see Table 5.3 ). There were

  only three cases identifi ed and they had no impact in the interview. No evi-

  dential use was made in the two cases that progressed to trial. Twenty - nine

  offi cers (58%) reported making use of the special warning procedure in previ-

  82

  Handbook of Psychology of Investigative Interviewing

  Table 5.3: Use and impact of a special warning ( SW )

  Use of SW

  Frequency

  Impact of SW in Interview

  Impact of SW in Court

  N = 187

  Yes

  3 (2%)

  0

  0

  No

  184 (98%)

  3

  2 *

  * One case acquitted.

  ous terrorist cases, but 24 (83%) claimed that it had no impact, and only one

  offi cer was aware of an example where an adverse inference was introduced at

  trial. It is of some concern that CT offi cers have very little faith in the probity

  of these valuable provisions.

  This raises an interesting methodological question: how many warnings

  should have been given? Unfortunately, this is not a straightforward issue. In

  the fi rst place the dataset is a biased selection – it contains only cases where

  people spoke and is therefore not representative. Secondly, if the suspect elects

  not to speak at the beginning of the interview but later does, can you assume

  that a SW should have been used immediately? It is obviously a question of

  judgement and one would need to be in possession of all the relevant facts

  and be alive to the many issues and context at that time. Perhaps a more pro-

  ductive approach would be to examine when a SW could and or should have

  been applied. With this criterion there was one case where a SW should have

  been given and a further three cases where one could have been applied.

  What is more worrying is the offi cers ’ lack of faith in these provisions and

  it needs to be established whether there is any substance to their claim that

  adverse inferences are simply not used to their full potential by the prosecu-

  tion. A recent Home Offi ce review of this area called for improved training

  and the need for greater awareness throughout the prosecution process, but

  no solid research was undertaken to determine the extent of the problem

  (personal communication from the Home Offi ce working party). The percep-

  tion exists that the loss of an indi
vidual ’ s right not to incriminate him - or

  herself was seen by the judiciary as an unacceptable erosion of individual

  freedom. Lord Carlile in his evidence to the Home Affairs Select Committee

  (14 February 2006) was dismissive of the value of the SW provision, which he

  labelled ‘ a fl eabite ’ when set against the inherent danger of a client actually

  answering a police offi cer ’ s questions in interview.

  Discussion

  The fi ndings of this study (only briefl y described here) indicate that the police

  interviewing of suspects in terrorist cases can best be described as polite, non -

  threatening and often non - productive. This research does not support the

  The Investigation of Terrorist Offences in the United Kingdom

  83

  fi ndings from the 1997 research in this fi eld in relation to the type and nature

  of interview tactics used by police in England and Wales in serious criminal

  cases.

  In order to make sense of these fi ndings we need to examine not only the

  many contextual issues associated with this interaction but also, within the

  confi nes of police detention, the actual climate in which such interactions take

  place.

  Contextual i ssues

  There is clearly a multilayered dimension to the contextual issues relevant to

  the investigation of terrorist offences and it will only be possible in this chapter

  to outline briefl y some of the many political, legal, psychological and organi-

  zational issues involved.

  The UK prime minister ’ s declaration that ‘ the rules of the game are chang-

  ing ’ succinctly captures the declared intent and extent of the reform process

  that the government is prepared to consider in order to defeat the threat posed

  by terrorist activity to this country (Tony Blair, cited by John Piennar at

  http://newsvote.bbc.co.uk/mpapps/pagetools/print/news/bbc.co.uk/1/

  hi/uk-politics/47 ). Blair ’ s reference to the ‘ rules of the game ’ is an unambigu-

  ous statement to give notice of the government

  ’ s intention to review the

  balance between civil liberties and security and to bring about the necessary

  changes to ensure the protection of the very democracy that protects our

  liberty.

  Further terrorist legislation has been announced that will usher in additional

  (and some retrospective) powers designed to create a more resolute environ-

  ment against extremist elements and those engaged in preaching or justifying

  hatred and violence. The option to detain a person without charge for up to

  28 days is likely to have a direct impact on the nature and dynamics of future

  police suspect interactions. It is perplexing that the period to detain terrorist

 

‹ Prev