Book Read Free

Not Born Yesterday

Page 16

by Hugo Mercier

of close personal ties for conversion has been repeatedly observed,

  from Mormons to Nichiren Shoshu Buddhists, or medieval

  Cathars.55

  Even if people are recruited by friends or family, conversion

  can entail some social costs inflicted by those not already con-

  verted, ranging from misunderstanding to persecution. In these

  conditions, doesn’t conversion reflect a feat of persuasion, get-

  ting someone to accept, on trust alone, a new set of beliefs, often

  accompanied by costly personal obligations? On the contrary,

  it seems that people who convert find something to their liking

  in their new group. Summarizing the lit er a ture on New Religious

  Movements, psychologist Dick Anthony notes that “the psycho-

  logical and emotional condition of most converts improves

  rather than declines after joining.”56 Even costly be hav iors can

  be beneficial. Mormons have to donate 10 percent of their in-

  come and 10 percent of their time to the church. Yet it is not too

  hard to see why some people would prefer to live in a commu-

  nity in which every one shares so much, enabling Mormons to

  124 ch ap t er 8

  “lavish social ser vices upon one another.”57 Even early Christians,

  who, at times, were at great risk of persecution, likely benefited

  from the support networks created by their adhesion to this new

  cult.58 By contrast with these practical aspects, the apparently

  exotic beliefs associated with new religions play a minor, post hoc

  role. As economist Laurence Iannaccone put it, “Belief typically

  follows involvement. Strong attachments draw people into reli-

  gious groups, but strong beliefs develop more slowly or never

  develop at all.”59

  New religious movements can grow by offering people a mode

  of social interaction they enjoy, without involving mass conver-

  sion. But what happens when a religion becomes ubiquitous or

  dominant? Aren’t its priests, then, able to dictate the people’s

  thoughts and be hav ior?

  Throughout the Middle Ages, the Catholic Church attempted

  to impose on the Eu ro pean peasantry be hav iors that weren’t in

  their obvious interest, from regular church attendance and con-

  fession all the way to the tithe, a 10 percent tax on what ever the

  peasants gathered each year. Moreover, the church also spread

  beliefs supporting the existing, iniquitous status quo. Kings had

  a divine right to rule. Priests taught that a view of the rich as

  merely lucky rather than deserving was “akin to covetousness,”

  the root of all evil.60

  This is what Marxist scholars have called the dominant ideol-

  ogy: a worldview created by the upper classes, justifying their

  position, that they impose on the rest of the population.61 For

  Marx and Engels, “The class which has the means of material

  production at its disposal, has control at the same time over the

  means of mental production, so that thereby, generally speaking,

  the ideas of those who lack the means of mental production are

  subject to it.”62 Getting wide swaths of the population to accept

  an ideology in which their misery is deserved, making them

  d e m a g o g ue s, p r o p he t s, p r e a c he r s 125

  resignedly accept their fate, would be the most impressive feat

  of mass persuasion ever accomplished.

  In line with this vision, the Catholic Church is often described

  as ruling supreme over the Eu ro pean Middle Ages. A mixture of

  deference, ignorance, and fear of hell would have enabled the

  church to make a sheepish population obey its injunctions and

  accept its doctrine.63 Humbert de Romans, a thirteenth- century

  Dominican who preached in poor areas of southern France, pro-

  vides a very diff er ent perspective on how this was working out

  for the church. Humbert was prob ably pretty good at his job—

  he later rose through the ranks to become the head of the Do-

  minican order— yet he despaired of what he saw on the ground.

  The church, for all its power, was barely able to get the poor

  to conform to the bare minimum of its doctrinal requirements:

  to be baptized, to know the “Our Father,” to take communion

  once a year.64 Humbert complains of people going to church only

  to “pass the night gossiping to each other, not only about vain

  subjects, but about evil and indecent ones.”65 What about spe-

  cial occasions? The flock did enjoy saints’ days, yet it wasn’t the

  church that benefited, but “inn keepers and prostitutes.”66 Believ-

  ers also went on pilgrimages, the occasion for “more sin, some-

  times, than a participant committed in all the rest of the year put

  together.”67

  The poor not only turned religious ceremonies into opportu-

  nities for debauchery but actively resisted any costly be hav ior

  the church attempted to impose. Humbert deplores “the neglect

  of penance or fasting” and “the reluctance to pay tithes.”68 His-

  torian Emmanuel Le Roy Ladurie notes how “the conflict over

  tithes . . . runs like a thread through peasant protests; it consti-

  tutes, from Catharism to Calvinism, a common denominator,

  more obvious than any dogmatic continuity, which is often

  absent.”69

  126 ch ap t er 8

  How do the people’s crusades fit into this picture of disobe-

  dience? Didn’t the church manage to persuade thousands of poor

  people to perform the ultimate sacrifice on its behalf? As men-

  tioned earlier, the poor often saw such crusades as an opportunity

  for pil aging, rather than a spiritual calling. In any case, these cru-

  sades weren’t a brainchild of the church hierarchy.70 Indeed, the

  church sometimes actively fought them, and for good reason, as

  the “eschatological y inspired hordes of the poor,” after they had

  looted any Jewish dwel ing in sight, “soon turned on the clergy.”71

  At the height of the first Shepherd’s crusade, “the murder of a

  priest was regarded as particularly praiseworthy.”72 It seems the

  shepherds didn’t get the memo about dominant ideology.

  As well as refusing to comply with the costly be hav iors the

  church demanded of them, the medieval masses also rejected

  the bulk of Catholic doctrinal orthodoxy. Historians of ideas

  have noted how, all the way to the Enlightenment, “deep- seated

  and per sis tent paganism frequently camouflaged with the most

  superficial veneer [of Chris tian ity].”73 In his meticulous study

  of a French vil age in the thirteenth century, Le Roy Ladurie

  notes a variety of decidedly not-so-Christian goings-on. Upon the

  death of a house hold head, other members keep his hair and nail

  clippings, “ bearers of vital energy,” through which the house

  absorbs some of the deceased’s magical properties. A girl’s first

  menstrual blood is saved to be used later as a love potion. Um-

  bilical cords are preciously preserved, as they are thought to

  help win lawsuits (no refunds allowed).74 It is not surprising

  that Humbert, our Dominican preacher, would reproach peas-

  ants for being “much prone to sortilege” and �
��so obstinate, nay

  even incorrigible, that they simply cannot be stopped, either by

  excommunications, or by any other kind of threat.”75

  Among the beliefs squarely rejected by the poor were those

  supposed to make them docilely accept their condition. Humbert

  d e m a g o g ue s, p r o p he t s, p r e a c he r s 127

  might have taught that covetousness was a sin, but he still

  remarked how “the masses are accustomed to regard the rich of

  this world as the lucky ones,” and are eager to complain about

  “the poor state of church government,” which they (often, right-

  fully enough) blame on “bad bishops.”76 Throughout the

  Middle Ages, the Catholic Church’s efforts to impose unappeal-

  ing beliefs and costly be hav iors on the population through mass

  persuasion are a litany of failures.

  The pattern observed in Catholic medieval Eu rope recurs in

  eco nom ically dominated classes throughout the world.77 Far

  from having imbibed the dominant ideology, everywhere people

  practice “the arts of re sis tance” with the “weapons of the weak,”

  to borrow the titles of two influential books by sociologist James

  Scott.78 Even the strongest of power asymmetries, that between

  masters and their slaves, cannot make the subordinates accept

  their plight, which slaves keep fighting by any means available,

  ranging from “foot dragging, dissimulation, false compliance,”

  all the way to “arson” and “sabotage.”79

  The dominant ideology thesis has a point: the dominant

  classes weave narratives of the status quo as the best of all pos-

  si ble worlds, their superior position well deserved. Oftentimes,

  these narratives crowd communication channels, from manu-

  scripts to airwaves. But this does not mean that people farther

  down the social ladder are buying any of it. On the contrary,

  these narratives are resisted everywhere, and alternative narratives

  created— including millenarian visions when an opportunity for

  revolution arises.

  9

  PROPAGANDISTS,

  CAMPAIGNERS,

  AND ADVERTISERS

  Propagandists

  While he was in jail, working on Mein Kampf, Hitler thought a

  lot about propaganda. He described the masses as credulous: a

  “crowd of human children,” “feminine in its character” ( children

  and woman had long been associated with emotionality and

  gul ibility). Accordingly, effective propaganda must rest on “ste-

  reo typed formulas . . . per sis tently repeated until the very last

  individual has come to grasp the idea that has been put forward.”1

  Once he had been elected— mostly on an economic, anticom-

  munist platform— and had consolidated his power, Hitler, with

  the help of Joseph Goebbels and the propaganda ministry, turned

  his theory into practice. Together, they would develop the most

  ignominious mass persuasion attempt in history, in par tic u lar as

  it aimed to build up German anti- Semitism, vilifying Jewish

  people in movies, radio programs, books, posters, educational

  materials, and so forth.

  How effective was this “propaganda barrage”?2 To obtain fine-

  grained data on German anti- Semitism, economists Nico

  Voigtländer and Hans- Joachim Voth looked at surveys from 1996

  128

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 129

  and 2006.3 They mea sured whether anti- Semitism is still higher

  among Germans who were exposed to the Nazi propaganda ma-

  chine, in par tic u lar those born in the 1920s and 1930s. They

  found that indeed these cohorts expressed stronger anti- Semitic

  sentiments: Germans born in the 1920s and 1930s agreed with

  statements such as “Jews have too much influence in the world”

  between 5 and 10 percent more than those born at any other time.

  Even if Nazi propaganda is responsible for only a small share

  of the anti- Semitism currently pre sent in Germany, it certainly

  seems to have contributed to the prob lem. But did it do so

  through brute repetition, as Hitler thought? Voigtländer and

  Voth looked at the regional variations in the availability of

  propaganda— how many people owned radios, the number of

  cinemas where they could watch propaganda films, and so forth.

  If mere repetition were effective, areas with greater exposure to

  propaganda should see the sharpest rise in anti- Semitism. In fact,

  the sheer exposure to propaganda had no effect at all. Instead,

  it was the presence of preexisting anti- Semitism that explained

  the regional variation in the effectiveness of propaganda. Only

  the areas that were the most anti- Semitic before Hitler came to

  power proved receptive. For people in these areas, the anti-

  Semitic propaganda might have been used as a reliable cue that

  the government was on their side, and thus that they could freely

  express their prejudices.4 Another study that focused on the ef-

  fects of radio broadcasts yielded even stronger results: radio

  propaganda was “effective in places where antisemitism was his-

  torically high,” but it had “a negative effect in places with histori-

  cally low antisemitism.”5

  Ian Kershaw, the historian we encountered in the last chapter

  scouring the rec ords of Nazi Germany to understand Hitler’s

  popularity, also analyzed the effectiveness of Nazi propaganda.

  He reached similar conclusions. The Germans didn’t heed the

  130 ch ap t er 9

  calls to boycott Jewish stores and to ostracize the Jews more

  generally. It was only through “terror and legal discrimina-

  tion” that the Nazis achieved “the economic (and increasingly

  social) exclusion of the Jews from German life.”6

  Kershaw argues that other dimensions of Nazi propaganda

  were even less effective than the attempts to turn all Germans

  into rabid anti- Semites. The push to enforce compulsory eutha-

  nasia of the handicapped was widely resisted.7 Attacks on com-

  munism appealed to those already on the right but were an “al-

  most unmitigated failure . . . among German industrial

  workers,” communism’s natu ral constituency.8 Indeed, Nazi pro-

  paganda failed to persuade the majority of German workers to

  contribute willingly to the war effort, many electing to resist

  through absenteeism.9 As soon as the war took a turn for the

  worst— after Sta lin grad in particular— messages from the pro-

  paganda ministry fell on deaf ears. Goebbels’s “unvarying mes-

  sage of victory was becoming monotonic and ignored by the

  public,” who trusted the BBC over official government pro-

  grams.10 Nazi propaganda even failed to generate much liking

  for the Nazi Party, whose local officials, often incompetent and

  corrupt, were universally despised.11 One of the great ironies of

  these conclusions is that Kershaw reached them partly through

  an examination of work done by the Sicherheitsdienst (SD), the

  Nazi intel igence agency. Some of its reports are indeed scath-

  ing, such as this note from the SD office in Schweinfurt (a small

 
city of central Germany): “Our propaganda encounters rejection

  everywhere among the population because it is regarded as

  wrong and lying.”12

  What about the German armies that fought to the death

  battles they were certain to lose? Aren’t they the ultimate proof

  of the effectiveness of Nazi propaganda? Studies of German sol-

  diers consistently show that “po liti cal values played a very

  p r o p a g a nd is t s, c a mp a i g ne r s, a d v e r t ise r s 131

  minor part in sustaining their motivation for combat.”13 Instead,

  as for soldiers everywhere, the main impetus came from the sup-

  port of the small group they were embedded in, people they

  had fought alongside for many years, and with whom shared

  hardships had created special bonds of loyalty.14 Fear also played

  a role: fear of being executed if a desertion attempt failed (as

  thousands of German soldiers were), fear of dying as a POW in

  the wrong hands (few POWs on the eastern front ever came

  back, while the relatively lenient treatment of POWs on the west-

  ern front prompted many desertions).15

  Kershaw, summing up his findings, notes how “the effective-

  ness of [Nazi] propaganda . . . was heavi ly dependent on its

  ability to build on existing consensus, to confirm existing values,

  to bolster existing prejudices.”16 Whenever propaganda ran

  against public opinion, it failed abysmally. On the whole, little

  or no mass persuasion took place. Were the Nazis just particu-

  larly bad at propaganda, or do we observe the same pattern in

  other regimes?

  The USSR also made abundant use of propaganda not only

  during the war but also in the preceding de cades, in par tic u lar

  when Stalin was consolidating his power. Early Soviet propa-

  ganda efforts failed to resonate with the population. Commu-

  nist concepts had to be jettisoned in favor of more congenial nar-

  ratives: patriotism replaced internationalism, the cult of heroes

  replaced impersonal historical forces.17 In turn, this strategy back-

  fired badly when many of the heroes were killed during the

  show trials of the late 1930s. Soviet propaganda never quite re-

  covered. Indeed, even at the apex of Stalinist propaganda,

  Rus sian workers and peasants “ adopted many tactics of passive

  re sis tance,” and actively sought out “alternative sources of in-

  formation.”18 As elsewhere, it is mostly those who benefited

 

‹ Prev