Book Read Free

Not Born Yesterday

Page 38

by Hugo Mercier


  of effect size.” International Journal of Advertising, 27(2), 209–234.

  Analytis, P. P., Barkoczi, D., & Herzog, S. M. (2018). “Social learning strategies for

  matters of taste.” Nature Human Behaviour, 2(6), 415–424.

  Anderson, C., Brion, S., Moore, D. A., & Kennedy, J. A. (2012). “A status- enhancement

  account of overconfidence.” Journal of Personality and Social Psy chol ogy, 103(4),

  718–735.

  Anthony, D. (1999). “Pseudoscience and minority religions: An evaluation of the

  brainwashing theories of Jean- Marie Abgrall.” Social Justice Research, 12(4),

  421–456.

  Arceneaux, K., & Johnson, M. (2013). Changing minds or changing channels? Partisan

  news in an age of choice. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Ardener, E. (1970). “Witchcraft, economics, and the continuity of belief.” In M. Doug-

  las (Ed.), Witchcraft confessions and accusations (pp. 141–160). London:

  Routledge.

  Arendt, H. (1963). Eichmann in Jerusalem: A report on the banality of evil. New York:

  Viking.

  Arika, N. (2007). Passions and tempers: A history of the humors. New York: Harper

  Perennial.

  Artés, J. (2013). “Do Spanish politicians keep their promises?” Party Politics, 19(1),

  143–158.

  Asch, S. E. (1956). “Studies of in de pen dence and conformity: A minority of one against

  a unan imous majority.” Psychological Monographs, 70(9), 1–70.

  r e f e r e n c e s 309

  Aveni, A. F. (1977). “The not- so- lonely crowd: Friendship groups in collective be hav-

  ior.” Sociometry, 40(1), 96–99.

  Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2019). “The smart System 1: Evidence for the intuitive na-

  ture of correct responding in the bat- and- ball prob lem.” Thinking & Reasoning,

  25(3), 257–299.

  Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2010).

  “Optimally interacting minds.” Science, 329(5995), 1081–1085.

  Balliet, D. (2010). “Communication and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-

  analytic review.” Journal of Conflict Resolution, 54(1), 39–57.

  Barabas, J., & Jerit, J. (2009). “Estimating the causal effects of media coverage on

  policy- specific knowledge.” American Journal of Po liti cal Science, 53(1), 73–89.

  Barker, E. (1984). The making of a Moonie: Choice or brainwashing? Oxford:

  Blackwel .

  Barker, J. (2014). 1381: The year of the Peasants’ Revolt. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-

  versity Press.

  Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind. Oxford: Oxford

  University Press.

  Barkun, M. (1986). Disaster and the millennium. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University

  Press.

  Barlev, M., Mermelstein, S., & German, T. C. (2017). “Core intuitions about persons

  coexist and interfere with acquired Christian beliefs about God.” Cognitive Science,

  41(53), 425–454.

  Barlev, M., Mermelstein, S., & German, T. C. (2018). “Repre sen ta tional coexistence

  in the God concept: Core knowledge intuitions of God as a person are not revised

  by Christian theology despite lifelong experience.” Psychonomic Bulletin and Re-

  view, 25(6) 1–9.

  Barrett, J. L. (1999). “Theological correctness: Cognitive constraint and the study of

  religion.” Method and Theory in the Study of Religion, 11(4), 325–339.

  Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). “Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropo-

  morphism in God concepts.” Cognitive Psy chol ogy, 31(3), 219–247.

  Barrows, S. (1981). Distorting mirrors: Visions of the crowd in late nineteenth- century

  France. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Baumard, N., André, J. B., & Sperber, D. (2013). “A mutualistic approach to morality:

  The evolution of fairness by partner choice.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1),

  59–78.

  Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013a). “Explaining moral religions.” Trends in Cognitive Sci-

  ences, 17(6), 272–280.

  Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013b). “Religious beliefs as reflective elaborations on in-

  tuitions: A modified dual- process model.” Current Directions in Psychological Sci-

  ence, 22(4), 295–300.

  310 references

  Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2015). “The nature and dynamics of world religions:

  A life- history approach.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282, 20151593. https://

  doi .org / 10 .1098 / rspb .2015 .1593

  Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I., & Boyer, P. (2015). “Increased affluence explains

  the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions.” Current Biology, 25(1),

  10–15.

  Beam, M. A., Hutchens, M. J., & Hmielowski, J. D. (2018). “Facebook news and (de)

  polarization: Reinforcing spirals in the 2016 US election.” Information, Commu-

  nication and Society, 21(7), 940–958.

  Bekkouche, Y., & Cagé, J. (2018). The price of a vote: Evidence from France, 1993–

  2014. Retrieved from https:// papers .ssrn .com / sol3 / papers .cfm ? abstract _ id

  = 3125220

  Benjamin, L. T., & Simpson, J. A. (2009). The power of the situation: The impact of

  Milgram’s obedience studies on personality and social psy chol ogy. American Psy-

  chologist, 64(1), 12–19.

  Benjamin, P., & Shapiro, R. (1992). The rational public: Fifty years of trends in Ameri-

  cans’ policy preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Benkler, Y., Faris, R., & Roberts, H. (2018). Network propaganda: Manipulation, dis-

  information, and radicalization in American politics. New York: Oxford University

  Press.

  Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., McPhee, W. N., & McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A

  study of opinion formation in a presidential campaign. Chicago: University of Chi-

  cago Press.

  Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). “Trust, reciprocity, and social history.”

  Games and Economic Be hav ior, 10(1), 122–142.

  Bergstrom, B., & Boyer, P. (submitted). Who mental systems believe: Effects of source

  on judgments of truth.

  Bergstrom, C. T., & Lachmann, M. (2001). “Alarm calls as costly signals of anti- predator

  vigilance: The watchful babbler game.” Animal Behaviour, 61(3), 535–543.

  Bernard, S., Proust, J., & Clément, F. (2015). “Four- to six- year- old children’s sensitiv-

  ity to reliability versus consensus in the endorsement of object labels.” Child

  Development, 86(4), 1112–1124.

  Berreman, G. D. (1971). “On the nature of caste in India: A review symposium on Louis

  Dumont’s Homo Hierarchicus: 3 The Brahmannical View of Caste.” Contributions

  to Indian Sociology, 5(1), 16–23.

  Besley, T., & Burgess, R. (2002). “The po liti cal economy of government responsive-

  ness: Theory and evidence from India.” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(4),

  1415–1451.

  Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2007). “The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false

  beliefs.” Psychological Science, 18(5), 382–386.

  r e f e r e n c e s 311

  Blake, T., Nosko, C., & Tadelis, S. (2015). “Consumer heterogeneity and paid search

  effectiveness: A large- scale field experiment.” Econometrica, 83(1), 155–174.

  Blakeslee, D. (2014). Propaganda and politics in developing countries: Evidence from India.

  Retrie
ved from https:// papers .ssrn .com / sol3 / papers .cfm ? abstract _ id = 2542702

  Blancke, S., Boudry, M., & Pigliucci, M. (2017). “Why do irrational beliefs mimic sci-

  ence? The cultural evolution of pseudoscience.” Theoria, 83(1), 78–97.

  Blumstein, D. T., Steinmetz, J., Armitage, K. B., & Daniel, J. C. (1997). “Alarm calling

  in yellow- bel ied marmots: II. The importance of direct fitness.” Animal Behaviour,

  53(1), 173–184.

  Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian be hav ior. Cam-

  bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Bonaccio, S., & Dalal, R. S. (2006). “Advice taking and decision- making: An integra-

  tive lit er a ture review, and implications for the orga nizational sciences.” Orga-

  nizational Be hav ior and Human Decision Pro cesses, 101(2), 127–151.

  Bond, C. F. (2008). “Commentary: A few can catch a liar, sometimes: Comments on

  Ekman and O’ Sullivan (1991), as well as Ekman, O’ Sullivan, and Frank (1999).”

  Applied Cognitive Psy chol ogy, 22(9), 1298–1300.

  Bond, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). “Accuracy of deception judgments.” Personal-

  ity and Social Psy chol ogy Review, 10(3), 214–234.

  Bond, C. F., Howard, A. R., Hutchison, J. L., & Masip, J. (2013). “Overlooking the obvi-

  ous: Incentives to lie.” Basic and Applied Social Psy chol ogy, 35(2), 212–221.

  Bonnefon, J.- F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2017). “Can we detect cooperators

  by looking at their face?” Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(3),

  276–281.

  Borgia, G. (1985). “Bower quality, number of decorations and mating success of male

  satin bowerbirds ( Ptilonorhynchus violaceus): An experimental analy sis.” Animal

  Behaviour, 33(1), 266–271.

  Borgia, G. (1993). “The cost of display in the non- resource- based mating system of

  the satin bowerbird.” American Naturalist, 141(5), 729–743.

  Boss, L. P. (1997). “Epidemic hysteria: A review of the published lit er a ture.” Epidemio-

  logic Reviews, 19(2), 233–243.

  Boxel , L., Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2017). “Greater internet use is not associ-

  ated with faster growth in po liti cal polarization among US demographic groups.”

  Proceedings of the National Acad emy of Sciences, 201706588.

  Boxel , L., Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2018). “A note on internet use and the 2016

  US presidential election outcome.” PloS One, 13(7), e0199571.

  Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary pro cess. Chicago: Uni-

  versity of Chicago Press.

  Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. New York:

  Oxford University Press.

  312 references

  Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Los An-

  geles: University of California Press.

  Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained. London: Heinemann.

  Boyer, P., & Baumard, N. (2018). “The diversity of religious systems across history.”

  In J. R. Liddle & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary

  psy chol ogy and religion (pp. 1–24). New York: Oxford University Press.

  Boyer, P., & Parren, N. (2015). “Threat- related information suggests competence: A

  pos si ble factor in the spread of rumors.” PloS One, 10(6), e0128421.

  Boyer, P., & Petersen, M. B. (2012). “The naturalness of (many) social institutions:

  Evolved cognition as their foundation.” Journal of Institutional Economics, 8(1), 1–25.

  Boyer, P., & Petersen, M. B. (2018). “Folk- economic beliefs: An evolutionary cogni-

  tive model.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e158.

  Brain, P. (1986). Galen on bloodletting: A study of the origins, development, and validity

  of his opinions, with a translation of the three works. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-

  versity Press.

  Brain, R. (1970). “Child- witches.” In M. Douglas (Ed.), Witchcraft confessions and ac-

  cusations (pp. 161–182). London: Routledge.

  Brand, C. O., & Mesoudi, A. (2018). “Prestige and dominance based hierarchies exist

  in naturally occurring human groups, but are unrelated to task- specific knowledge.”

  Royal Society Open Science, 6(6), 181621. https:// doi .org / 10 .1098 / rsos .181621

  Brandenberger, D. (2012). Propaganda state in crisis: Soviet ideology, indoctrination, and

  terror under Stalin, 1927–1941. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Braucher, J., & Orbach, B. (2015). “Scamming: The misunderstood confidence man.”

  Yale Journal of Law and the Humanities, 27(2), 249–287.

  Brennan, J. (2012). The ethics of voting. New York: Prince ton University Press.

  Brennan, J. (2016). Against democracy. Prince ton, NJ: Prince ton University Press.

  Broockman, D. E., & Butler, D. M. (2017). “The causal effects of elite position- taking

  on voter attitudes: Field experiments with elite communication.” American Jour-

  nal of Po liti cal Science, 61(1), 208–221.

  Broockman, D. E., & Green, D. P. (2014). “Do online advertisements increase po liti-

  cal candidates’ name recognition or favorability? Evidence from randomized field

  experiments.” Po liti cal Be hav ior, 36(2), 263–289.

  Brosseau- Liard, P. E., & Poulin- Dubois, D. (2014). “Sensitivity to confidence cues in-

  creases during the second year of life.” Infancy, 19(5), 461–475.

  Brown, R. (1965). Social psy chol ogy. New York: Free Press.

  Buckner, H. T. (1965). “A theory of rumor transmission.” Public Opinion Quarterly,

  29(1), 54–70.

  Burger, J. M., Girgis, Z. M., & Manning, C. C. (2011). “In their own words: Explaining

  obedience to authority through an examination of participants’ comments.” So-

  cial Psychological and Personality Science, 2(5), 460–466.

  r e f e r e n c e s 313

  Burgess, T. H. (1839). The physiology or mechanism of blushing. London: Churchil .

  Burns, J. (2012). “Race, diversity and pro- social be hav ior in a segmented society.” Jour-

  nal of Economic Be hav ior and Organ ization, 81(2), 366–378.

  Burridge, K. O. L. (1972). “Tangu.” In P. Lawrence & M. J. Meggitt (Eds.), Gods, ghosts

  and men in Melanesia: Some religions of Australian New Guinea and the New Heb-

  rides (pp. 224–249). New York: Oxford University Press.

  Bursztyn, L., Egorov, G., & Fiorin, S. (2019). From extreme to mainstream: The erosion

  of social norms. https:// home .uchicago .edu / bursztyn / Bursztyn _ Egorov _ Fiorin

  _ Extreme _ Mainstream _ 2019 _ 06 _ 05 .pdf.

  Campagna, R. L., Mislin, A. A., Kong, D. T., & Bottom, W. P. (2016). “Strategic con-

  sequences of emotional misrepre sen ta tion in negotiation: The blowback effect.”

  Journal of Applied Psy chol ogy, 101(5), 605–624.

  Canetti, E. (1981). Crowds and power (C. Stewart, Trans.). New York: Noonday

  Press.

  Caplow, T. (1947). “Rumors in war.” Social Forces, 25(3), 298–302.

  Carlsson, M., Dahl, G. B., & Rooth, D.- O. (2015). Do politicians change public attitudes?

  NBER Working Paper No. 21062. Retrieved from https:// www .nber .org / papers

  / w21062

  Caro, T. M. (1986a). “The functions of stotting: A review of the hypotheses.” Animal

  Behaviour, 34(3), 649–662.

  Caro, T. M. (1986b). “The functions of stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: Some tests of

  the p
redictions.” Animal Behaviour, 34(3), 663–684.

  Carruthers, S. L. (2009). Cold War captives: Imprisonment, escape, and brainwashing.

  Los Angeles: University of California Press.

  Castelain, T., Bernard, S., & Mercier, H. (2018). “Evidence that two- year- old children

  are sensitive to information presented in arguments.” Infancy, 23(1), 124–135.

  Castelain, T., Bernard, S., Van der Henst, J.- B., & Mercier, H. (2016). “The influence

  of power and reason on young Maya children’s endorsement of testimony.” De-

  velopmental Science, 19(6), 957–966.

  Castelain, T., Girotto, V., Jamet, F., & Mercier, H. (2016). “Evidence for benefits of ar-

  gumentation in a Mayan indigenous population.” Evolution and Human Be hav ior,

  37(5), 337–342.

  Cavari, A., & Freedman, G. (2018). “Polarized mass or polarized few? Assessing the

  parallel rise of survey nonresponse and mea sures of polarization.” Journal of Poli-

  tics, 80(2), 719–725.

  Chagnon, N. A. (1992). Yanomamö: The fierce people (4th ed.). New York: Holt, Rine-

  hart and Winston.

  Chan, M. S., Jones, C. R., Hall Jamieson, K., & Albarracin, D. (2017). Debunking: A

  meta- analysis of the psychological efficacy of messages countering misinforma-

  tion. Psychological Science, 28(11), 1531–1546.

  314 references

  Chanel, O., Luchini, S., Massoni, S., & Vergnaud, J.- C. (2011). “Impact of information

  on intentions to vaccinate in a potential epidemic: Swine- origin influenza A

  (H1N1).” Social Science and Medicine, 72(2), 142–148.

  Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). “Promises and partnership.” Econometrica,

  74(6), 1579–1601.

  Cheatham, M. L. (2008). “The death of George Washington: An end to the contro-

  versy?” American Surgeon, 74(8), 770–774.

  Chen, X., & Shi, T. (2001). “Media effects on po liti cal confidence and trust in the

  People’s Republic of China in the post- Tiananmen period.” East Asia, 19(3),

  84–118.

  Chen, Y., YeckehZaare, I., & Zhang, A. F. (2018). “Real or bogus: Predicting suscep-

  tibility to phishing with economic experiments.” PloS One, 13(6), e0198213.

  Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). “Assessment of meaning and the detection

  of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys.” Animal Behaviour, 36(2), 477–486.

  Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world. Chicago: Uni-

 

‹ Prev