Book Read Free

Not Born Yesterday

Page 41

by Hugo Mercier

ner (Ed.), Handbook of social psy chol ogy (pp. 244–252). Cambridge MA:

  Addison- Wesley.

  Howard, G. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic

  Books.

  Hu, J., Whalen, A., Buchsbaum, D., Griffiths, T., & Xu, F. (2015). “Can children bal-

  ance the size of a majority with the quality of their information?” Proceedings of

  the Cognitive Science Society Conference. Pasadena, California, July 22–25.

  Hu, Y., Lodish, L. M., & Krieger, A. M. (2007). “An analy sis of real world TV adver-

  tising tests: A 15- year update.” Journal of Advertising Research, 47(3), 341–353.

  Huang, H. (2017). “A war of (mis)information: The po liti cal effects of rumors and

  rumor rebuttals in an authoritarian country.” British Journal of Po liti cal Science,

  47(2), 283–311.

  Huckfeldt, R. (2001). “The social communication of po liti cal expertise.” American Jour-

  nal of Po liti cal Science, 45(2), 425–438.

  Huckfeldt, R., Pietryka, M. T., & Reilly, J. (2014). “Noise, bias, and expertise in po-

  liti cal communication networks.” Social Networks, 36, 110–121.

  Hutton, R. (2017). The witch: A history of fear, from ancient times to the pre sent. New

  Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Iannaccone, L. R. (1992). “Sacrifice and stigma: Reducing free- riding in cults, com-

  munes, and other collectives.” Journal of Po liti cal Economy, 100(2), 271–291.

  Iannaccone, L. R. (2006). “The market for martyrs.” Interdisciplinary Journal of Research

  on Religion, 2(4), 1–28.

  Inbau, F., Reid, J., Buckley, J., & Jayne, B. (2001). Criminal interrogation and confessions

  (4th ed.). Gaithersberg, MD: Aspen.

  Isberner, M.- B., & Richter, T. (2013). “Can readers ignore implausibility? Evidence

  for nonstrategic monitoring of event- based plausibility in language comprehen-

  sion.” Acta Psychologica, 142(1), 15–22.

  Isberner, M.- B., & Richter, T. (2014). “Does validation during language comprehen-

  sion depend on an evaluative mindset?” Discourse Pro cesses, 51(1–2), 7–25.

  Isenberg, D. J. (1986). “Group polarization: A critical review and meta- analysis.” Jour-

  nal of Personality and Social Psy chol ogy, 50(6), 1141–1151.

  Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters: Tele vi sion and public opinion.

  Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  r e f e r e n c e s 327

  Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N., & Westwood, S. J. (2019). “The

  origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States.” Annual

  Review of Po liti cal Science, 22, 129–146.

  Janis, I. L. (1951). Air war and emotional stress: Psychological studies of bombing and civil-

  ian defense. New York: McGraw- Hil .

  Jeffries, S. (2016). Grand hotel abyss: The lives of the Frankfurt School. New York: Verso.

  Jiménez, Á. V., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). “Prestige- biased social learning: Current evi-

  dence and outstanding questions.” Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 20. Retrieved

  from https:// www .nature .com / articles / s41599 - 019 - 0228 - 7

  Jo, D. (2017). Better the devil you know: An online field experiment on news consumption.

  Retrieved from https:// bfi .uchicago .edu / sites / default / files / research / Better _ the

  _ Devil _ You _ Know _ Online _ Field _ Experiment _ on _ News _ Consumption - 2 .pdf

  Johnson, N. R. (1988). “Fire in a crowded theater: A descriptive investigation of the

  emergence of panic.” International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 6(1),

  7–26.

  Johnson, N. R., Feinberg, W. E., & Johnston, D. M. (1994). “Microstructure and panic:

  The impact of social bonds on individual action in collective flight from the Bev-

  erly Hills Supper Club fire.” In R. R. Dynes & K. J. Tierney (Eds.), Disasters,

  collective be hav ior and social organ izations (pp. 168–189). Newark: University of

  Delaware Press.

  Jordan, S., Brimbal, L., Wallace, D. B., Kassin, S. M., Hartwig, M., & Street, C. N. (In

  press). “A test of the micro-expressions training tool: Does it improve lie detection?”

  Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1002

  /jip.1532

  Juergensmeyer, M. (1980). “What if the Untouchables don’t believe in Untouchabil-

  ity?” Bulletin of Concerned Asian Scholars, 12(1), 23–28.

  Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

  Kalla, J. L., & Broockman, D. E. (2018). “The minimal persuasive effects of campaign

  contact in general elections: Evidence from 49 field experiments.” American Po-

  liti cal Science Review, 112(1), 148–166.

  Kallis, A. (2008). Nazi propaganda in the Second World War. London: Palgrave

  Macmillan.

  Kam, C. D., & Zechmeister, E. J. (2013). “Name recognition and candidate support.”

  American Journal of Po liti cal Science, 57(4), 971–986.

  Kanwisher, N. (2000). “Domain specificity in face perception.” Nature Neuroscience,

  3(8), 759–763.

  Kaplan, S. L. (1982). Le complot de famine: Histoire d’une rumeur au XVIIIe siècle (Vol.

  39). Paris: A. Colin.

  Karsh, E., & Rautsi, I. (2007). Saddam Hussein: A po liti cal biography. New York: Grove/

  Atlantic.

  328 references

  Kassin, S. M., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (2004). “The psy chol ogy of confessions: A re-

  view of the lit er a ture and issues.” Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(2),

  33–67.

  Kassin, S. M., Meissner, C. A., & Norwick, R. J. (2005). “ ‘I’d know a false confession

  if I saw one’: A comparative study of college students and police investigators.” Law

  and Human Be hav ior, 29(2), 211–227.

  Kassin, S. M., & Neumann, K. (1997). “On the power of confession evidence: An ex-

  perimental test of the fundamental difference hypothesis.” Law and Human Be-

  hav ior, 21(5), 469–484.

  Kassin, S. M., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1980). “Prior confessions and mock juror verdicts.”

  Journal of Applied Social Psy chol ogy, 10(2), 133–146.

  Katz, E. (1957). “The two- step flow of communication: An up- to- date report on an

  hypothesis.” Public Opinion Quarterly, 21(1), 61–78.

  Katz, E., & Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1955). Personal influence: The part played by people in the

  flow of mass communications. Glencoe: Free Press.

  Kay, J. (2011). Among the Truthers: A journey through Amer ica’s growing conspiracist

  under ground. New York: HarperCol ins.

  Keil, F. C., Stein, C., Webb, L., Bil ings, V. D., & Rozenblit, L. (2008). “Discerning the

  division of cognitive labor: An emerging understanding of how knowledge is clus-

  tered in other minds.” Cognitive Science, 32(2), 259–300.

  Keller, K. L., & Lehmann, D. R. (2006). “Brands and branding: Research findings and

  future priorities.” Marketing Science, 25(6), 740–759.

  Kennedy, J. A., Anderson, C., & Moore, D. A. (2013). “When overconfidence is re-

  vealed to others: Testing the status- enhancement theory of overconfidence.” Orga-

  nizational Be hav ior and Human Decision Pro cesses, 122(2), 266–279.

  Kennedy, J. J. (2009). “Maintaining popu lar support for the Chinese Communist

  Party: The influence of education and the state- controlled media.” Po liti cal Stud-
/>   ies, 57(3), 517–536.

  Kershaw, I. (1983a). “How effective was Nazi propaganda?” In D. Welch (Ed.), Nazi

  propaganda: The power and the limitations (pp. 180–205). London: Croom Helm.

  Kershaw, I. (1983b). Popu lar opinion and po liti cal dissent in the Third Reich, Bavaria

  1933–1945. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Kershaw, I. (1987). The “Hitler myth”: Image and real ity in the Third Reich. New York:

  Oxford University Press.

  Kershaw, I. (1991). Hitler: Profiles in power. London: Routledge.

  Khare, R. S. (1984). The untouchable as himself: Ideology, identity and pragmatism among

  the Lucknow Chamars (Vol. 8). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Kierkegaard, S. (1961). Diary (P. P. Rohde, Ed.). London: Peter Owen.

  Kim, G., & Kwak, K. (2011). “Uncertainty matters: Impact of stimulus ambiguity on

  infant social referencing.” Infant and Child Development, 20(5), 449–463.

  r e f e r e n c e s 329

  Kim, J. W. (2018). Evidence can change partisan minds: Rethinking the bounds of moti-

  vated reasoning. Working paper.

  Kim, J. W., & Kim, E. (in press). “Identifying the effect of po liti cal rumor diffusion

  using variations in survey timing.” Quarterly Journal of Po liti cal Science.

  King, G., Pan, J., & Roberts, M. E. (2017). “How the Chinese government fabricates

  social media posts for strategic distraction, not engaged argument.” American Po-

  liti cal Science Review, 111(3), 484–501.

  Kitcher, P. (1993). The advancement of science: Science without legend, objectivity with-

  out illusions. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Klapper, J. T. (1960). The effects of mass communication. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

  Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., & Ryan, J. B. (2018). “Affective polarization or partisan disdain?

  Untangling a dislike for the opposing party from a dislike of partisanship.” Public

  Opinion Quarterly, 82(2), 379–390.

  Klarman, M. J. (2016). The framers’ coup: The making of the United States Constitution.

  New York: Oxford University Press.

  Knapp, R. H. (1944). “A psy chol ogy of rumor.” Public Opinion Quarterly, 8(1),

  22–37.

  Knittel, C. R., & Stango, V. (2009). Shareholder value destruction following the Tiger

  Woods scandal. University of California. Retrieved from Faculty. Gsm. Ucdavis.

  Edu/∼ Vstango/Tiger004. PdfKoch

  Kramer, A. D., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). “Experimental evidence of

  massive- scale emotional contagion through social networks.” Proceedings of the

  National Acad emy of Sciences, 201320040.

  Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). “Animal signals: Mind- reading and manipulation?”

  In J. Krebs, R., & Davies, N. B. (Eds.), Behavioural ecol ogy: An evolutionary approach

  (Vol. 2, pp. 390–402). Oxford: Basil Blackwell Scientific Publications.

  Kuller, C. (2015). “The demonstrations in support of the Protestant provincial bishop

  Hans Meiser: A successful protest against the Nazi regime.” In N. Stoltzfus & B.

  Maier- Katkin (Eds.), Protest in Hitler’s “National Community”: Popu lar unrest and

  the Nazi response (pp. 38–54). New York: Berghahn.

  Kurzban, R., & Christner, J. (2011). “Are super natural beliefs commitment devices

  for intergroup conflict?” In J. P. Forgas, A. Kruglanski, & K. D. Wil imas (Eds.), The

  psy chol ogy of social conflict and aggression (pp. 285–300). Sydney Symposium of

  Social Psy chol ogy, vol. 13). New York: Taylor and Francis.

  Kushnir, T., Vredenburgh, C., & Schneider, L. A. (2013). “ ‘Who can help me fix this

  toy?’: The distinction between causal knowledge and word knowledge guides pre-

  schoolers’ selective requests for information.” Developmental Psy chol ogy, 49(3),

  446–453.

  Lacan, J. (1939). “De l’impulsion au complexe.” Revue Française de Psychanalyse, 1,

  137–141.

  330 references

  Lacan, J. (1970). Of structure as an inmixing of an otherness prerequisite to any subject

  what ever (R. Macksey & E. Donato, Eds.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University

  Press.

  Lacan, J. (1980). De la Psychose paranoïaque dans ses rapports avec la personnalité. Paris:

  Seuil.

  Lacan, J. (2005). Le Séminaire, Livre 23, le sinthome. Paris: Seuil.

  Ladd, J. M. (2011). Why Americans hate the media and how it matters. New York: Prince-

  ton University Press.

  Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2009). “Exploiting a rare communication shift to docu-

  ment the persuasive power of the news media.” American Journal of Po liti cal Sci-

  ence, 53(2), 394–410.

  Lagrange, P. (2005). La guerre des mondes at- elle eu lieu? Paris: Robert Laffont.

  Laland, K. N. (2004). “Social learning strategies.” Animal Learning and Be hav ior, 32(1),

  4–14.

  Lanternari, V. (1963). The religions of the oppressed: A study of modern messianic cults.

  New York: Knopf.

  Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). “Expectations of cooperation and competition

  and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses.” Journal of Personality

  and Social Psy chol ogy, 56(4), 543–554.

  Larrick, R. P., & Sol , J. B. (2006). “Intuitions about combining opinions: Misappre-

  ciation of the averaging princi ple.” Management Science, 52, 111–127.

  Larson, H. J. (2018). “The biggest pandemic risk? Viral misinformation.” Nature,

  562(7727), 309–309.

  Lasswel , H. D. (1927). Propaganda technique in the world war. Cambridge, MA: MIT

  Press.

  Laustsen, L., & Bor, A. (2017). “The relative weight of character traits in po liti cal can-

  didate evaluations: Warmth is more impor tant than competence, leadership and

  integrity.” Electoral Studies, 49, 96–107.

  Lawlor, H. J., & Oulton, J. E. L. (1928). The ecclesiastical history and the martyrs of Pal-

  estine: Introduction, notes and index (Vol. 2). London: Society for Promoting

  Christian Knowledge.

  Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., & Gaudet, H. (1948). The people’s choice: How the voter

  makes up his mind in a presidential campaign. New York: Columbia University

  Press.

  Le Bon, G. (1897). The crowd: A study of the popu lar mind. London: Macmil ian.

  Le Bon, G. (1900). Psychologie des foules. Paris: Alcan.

  Le Bras, G. (1955). Etudes de sociologie religieuse. Paris: Presses Universitaires de

  France.

  Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2015). Can citizens be framed? How information, not em-

  phasis, changes opinions. Unpublished manuscript, Aarhus University.

  r e f e r e n c e s 331

  Leese, D. (2011). Mao cult: Rhe toric and ritual in China’s Cultural Revolution. Cam-

  bridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Lenz, G. S. (2009). “Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the

  priming hypothesis.” American Journal of Po liti cal Science, 53(4), 821–837.

  Lenz, G. S. (2013). Follow the leader? How voters respond to politicians’ policies and per-

  for mance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  Le Roy Ladurie, E. (2016). Montaillou, village occitan de 1294 à 1324. Paris: Editions

  Gallimard.

  Levendusky, M. S., & Malhotra, N. (2015). “(Mis)perceptions of partisan polarization

  in the American public.” Public Opinion Quarterly, 80(S1), 378–391.

  Lev
ine, T. R. (2014). “Truth- default theory (TDT): A theory of human deception

  and deception detection.” Journal of Language and Social Psy chol ogy, 33(4),

  378–392.

  Lévi- Strauss, C. (1967). “The sorcerer and his magic.” In J. Middleton (Ed.), Magic,

  witchcraft, and curing (pp. 23–42). New York: Natu ral History Press.

  Lévi- Strauss, C. (1986). “Entretien avec Judith Miller et Alain Grosrichard.” L’Ane. Le

  Magazine Freudien, 20, 27–29.

  Lewis, R. A., & Rao, J. M. (2013). On the near impossibility of mea sur ing the returns to

  advertising. Unpublished paper, Google, Inc. and Microsoft Research. Retrieved

  from http:// justinmrao .com / lewis _ rao _ nearimpossibility .pdf

  Liberman, V., Minson, J. A., Bryan, C. J., & Ross, L. (2012). “Naïve realism and cap-

  turing the ‘wisdom of dyads.’ ” Journal of Experimental Social Psy chol ogy, 48(2),

  507–512.

  Linton, R. (1963). Acculturation in seven American Indian tribes. New York: Peter

  Smith.

  Lloyd, G., & Sivin, N. (2002). The way and the word: Science and medicine in early China

  and Greece. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Lodish, L. M., Abraham, M., Kalmenson, S., Livelsberger, J., Lubetkin, B., Richard-

  son, B., & Stevens, M. E. (1995). “How TV advertising works: A meta- analysis of

  389 real world split cable TV advertising experiments.” Journal of Marketing Re-

  search, 32(2), 125–139.

  Lopez- Ibor, J. J., Soria, J., Canas, F., & Rodriguez- Gamazo, M. (1985). “Psychopatho-

  logical aspects of the toxic oil syndrome catastrophe.” British Journal of Psychiatry,

  147(4), 352–365.

  Luke, T. J. (in press). “Lessons from Pinocchio: Cues to deception may be highly exag-

  gerated.” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1745691619838258. https:// doi .org

  / 10 .1177 / 1745691619838258

  Lull, R. B., & Bushman, B. J. (2015). “Do sex and vio lence sel ? A meta- analytic review

  of the effects of sexual and violent media and ad content on memory, attitudes,

  and buying intentions.” Psychological Bulletin, 141(5), 1022–1048.

  332 references

  Lutz, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (2002). “Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor.”

  Child Development, 73(4) 1073–1084.

  Macfarlane, A. (1970). “Witchcraft in Tudor and Stuart Essex.” In M. Douglas (Ed.),

  Witchcraft confessions and accusations (pp. 81–101). London: Routledge.

 

‹ Prev