Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors
Page 6
According to a famous story in Herodotus’s History, an Egyptian king tried to ascertain the nature of the first language by isolating two children from birth and waiting to see in what tongue they first spoke. Study of the two new sign languages confirms that King Psammetichus’s experiment was misconceived: it is not specific words that are innate, but rather the systems for generating syntax and vocabulary. Among both the Nicaraguan schoolkids and the Al-Sayyid clan, a spontaneous sense of syntax has developed, specifically the distinction as to whether a word is the subject or object of a verb.
In addition, the Al-Sayyid signers have developed the preference for a specific order of words in a sentence, that of subject-object-verb. The Nicaraguan children, in contrast, have developed the signed equivalents of case endings for words. Since these indicate whether a word is subject or object, word order is not so important and keeps changing with each cohort of children.
The apparently spontaneous emergence of word order and case endings in the two sign languages strongly suggests that the basic elements of syntax are innate and generated by genetically specified components of the human brain. The Al-Sayyid sign language has been developed through only three generations but some signs have already become symbolic. The sign for man is a twirl of the finger to indicate a curled mustache, even though the men of the village no longer wear them. Change is also brisk in Nicaragua. At first the children represented the number twenty by flicking the fingers of both hands in the air twice, says Ann Senghas, a linguist who has been studying their sign language for 15 years. But the sign was too cumbersome and has now been replaced with a form, signable with one hand, that looks nothing like a 20 but can be signed fast.49
Sign languages emphasize an often overlooked aspect of language, that gesture is an integral accompaniment of the spoken word. The human proto-language doubtless included gestures, and could even have started with gestures alone. Michael Corballis, a psychologist at the University of Auckland in New Zealand, argues that language “developed first as a primarily gestural system, involving movements of the body, and more especially the hands, arms and face.”50 Speech would have evolved only later, he believes, because considerable evolutionary change had to occur to develop the fine muscles of the tongue and other parts of the vocal apparatus.
Corballis’s idea has several attractive features. It would explain why word and gesture are so well integrated, and why people even gesticulate when talking on the telephone despite the fact that their listeners cannot appreciate the performance. But critics of the idea note that gesture-based languages would be useless in the dark and that they require those conducting a conversation to be looking at each other all the time. Spoken language suffers from neither constraint.
Evolutionary Pressures for Language
Once language started, whether in the form of word or gesture or both, its further evolution would doubtless have been rapid because of the great advantages that each improvement in this powerful faculty would have conferred on its possessors. Even while still in its most rudimentary form, language would have made possible a whole new level of social interactions. Precise and unambiguous thoughts could at last be shared among members of a community, whether for making alliances, indicating intention, describing people and places, or transmitting knowledge. Moreover each small improvement in the overall system, whether in precision of hearing or articulation or syntax formation, would confer further benefit, and the genes underlying the change would sweep through the population.
But easy as it is to see how a simple form of language might have evolved into a complex one, that doesn’t answer the question of what particular stimulus brought language into being in the first place.
Language now plays so many roles in human society that it’s hard to arrange them in some hierarchy and say one role was the root and the others its branches. But evolutionary psychologists have come up with several interesting suggestions about the possible pressures for language to evolve. Robin Dunbar at the University of Liverpool in England has proposed a “social grooming” theory of language. He notes that monkeys and apes spend an inordinate amount of time grooming each other’s fur. This activity, besides curbing parasites, serves to cement social relationships. But social grooming sets a limit on the size of a monkey group, because members will have no time to search for food if there are too many acquaintances whose fur must be rubbed the right way.
In practice, different monkey species spend varying amounts of time on grooming one another, up to a maximum of 20% of their waking day, and this is among species whose typical group size is about 50 members. The maximum time available for social grooming, Dunbar argues, has effectively capped the size of monkey social groups at 50 members. How then did the typical size of hunter-gatherer groups grow to 150 members, a number that would in principle require everyone to spend 43% of their waking hours on social grooming, or its human equivalent? Because of language, Dunbar suggests. Language is so much more efficient a way of establishing and confirming social bonds that the requisite amount of social grooming could be cut way back. In a wide range of human societies, it so happens, the amount of time people spend in social interaction, or conversation, is 20%. The driving force behind the evolution of language, in Dunbar’s view, was the need to bond people in larger social groups.51
A quite different explanation has been advanced by the evolutionary psychologist Geoffrey Miller. He believes that sexual selection—Darwin’s theory that the peacock’s tail is the evolutionary product of peahens’ choices—is what has driven the evolution of language. Just as the richness and symmetry of the peacock’s tail signals its freedom from parasites, so eloquence and articulate speech signal the quality of an individual’s mind, and will be highly favored by both men and women in their sexual partners. Language is a device that lets us learn about potential mates more thoroughly than any other method, Miller writes.52
The Dunbar and Miller hypotheses are both evocative and each may hold some measure of truth. But it’s not clear if either really accounts for the richness and precision of language. Most adult speakers of English have a vocabulary of 60,000 words, though the top 4,000 words account for 98% of conversation. Does one really need 60,000 words, or even 4,000, for the purposes of social grooming, or even impressing one’s inamorata? Miller’s answer is that excess is the hallmark of sexual selection—once selection has started, the character under selection is taken to extremes, like the stag’s enormous antlers.
But for linguists, the essence of language is meaning and communication, and it seems unsatisfactory to explain its evolution on any other grounds. Pinker argues one should take into account the new ecological niche that humans had moved into, which was in fact a knowledge-laden environment requiring a wealth of new information about plants and animals, about how to make tools and weapons, and about goings-on in one’s own society. People’s longer life span made it worthwhile to gather information and transmit it to one’s children and grandchildren. “Language,” Pinker says, “meshes neatly with the other features of the cognitive niche. The zoologi cally unusual features of Homo sapiens can be explained parsimoniously by the idea that humans have evolved an ability to encode information about the causal structure of the world and to share it among themselves. Our hy persociality comes about because information is a particularly good commodity of exchange that makes it worth people’s while to hang out together.”
Pinker concludes that know-how, sociality and language are three key features of the distinctively human lifestyle and that the three factors coevolved, each acting as a selective pressure for the others.53
It would be easier to pinpoint the most likely stimulus for the evolution of human language if one could identify when language emerged. Obviously the joint human-chimp ancestor did not speak, or chimps would too. And all human races can speak equally well, so that fully articulate, modern language must have evolved before modern humans left Africa. This means language would have emerged after 5 million years ago
and before 50,000 years ago. Paleoanthropologists have made strenuous attempts to pin down the development of language through anatomy, by looking at the shape of the brain as implied by interior casts of old skulls, or features such as the hy oid, a U-shaped bone that supports the tongue muscles, and the hypoglossal canal, a passageway through bone for the nerve bundle that wires up the tongue muscles. But these studies have not yet brought a great deal of clarity to the problem.
Paleoanthropologists have tended to favor the idea that language started early, with Homo erectus or even the australopithecines, followed by slow and stately evolution. Archaeologists, on the other hand, tend to equate full-fledged modern language with art, which only becomes common in the archaeological record some 45,000 years ago. Their argument is that creation of art implies symbolic thinking in the mind of the artist, and therefore possession of language to share these abstract ideas.
Other archaeological facts favor a late start for language. To look at the rough stone tools of the Olduwan (made between 2.5 and 1.7 million years ago) they seem to be just chipped pebbles, made with no particular design in mind. But the tools of the Upper Paleolithic, which began 45,000 years ago, are precisely shaped and so well differentiated from each other that it seems plausible their makers had a different word for each, and therefore had language. “It is as though Upper Paleolithic flint workers were saying ‘This is an end-scraper: I use it as an end-scraper, I call it an end-scraper and it must therefore look like an end-scraper,’” writes the archaeologist Paul Mellars. He argues that the makers’ evident emphasis on the precise visual shape of their tools “is probably exactly what one would anticipate if Upper Paleolithic groups had a much more complex and highly structured vocabulary for the different artifact forms.” Given their much cruder tool kit, the Neanderthals might also have had language, Mellars thinks, but with a much simpler vocabulary.54
If fully articulate modern language emerged only 50,000 years ago, just before modern humans broke out of Africa, then the proto-language suggested by Bickerton would have preceded it. When might that proto-language first have appeared? If Homo ergaster possessed proto-language then so too would all its descendants, including the archaic hominids who reached the Far East (Homo erectus) and Europe (the Neanderthals). But in that case the Neanderthals, to judge by their lack of modern behavior, appear never to have developed their proto-language into fully modern articulate speech. That might seem surprising, given the advantage any improvement in the language faculty would confer on its owner, and the rapidity with which language might therefore be expected to evolve. So perhaps the Neanderthals didn’t speak at all.
Discovery of a Gene for Language
A remarkable new line of inquiry bearing on the origins of language has recently been opened up by the human genome project. This is the discovery of a gene that is intimately involved in many of the finer aspects of language. The gene, with the odd name of FOXP2, shows telltale signs of having changed significantly in humans but not in chimps, exactly as would be expected for a gene serving some new faculty that had emerged only in the human lineage. And, through the ability of genetics to reach back into the distant past, the emergence of the new gene can be dated, though at present only very roughly.
FOXP2 came to light through the discovery by Jane Hurst, an English geneticist, of an unusual London family whose existence she reported in 1990. The family consists of three generations. Of the 37 members old enough to be tested, 15 have a severe language deficit. Their speech is hard to understand, and they themselves have difficulty comprehending the speech of others. If asked to repeat a phrase like “pattaca pattaca pattaca,” they will stumble over each word as if it were entirely new. They have difficulty with a standard test of the ability to form past tenses of verbs (“Every day I wash my clothes, yesterday I_____my clothes”; four-year-olds will say “washed” as soon as they get the idea). They have problems in writing as well as speaking. The affected members of the family have been given intensive speech training but mostly hold jobs where not much talking is required. “Their speech is difficult to understand, particularly over the telephone, or if the context is not known. In a group of family members it is hard for you to pick up the pieces of the conversation, which is difficult to follow because many of the words are not correctly pronounced,” says Faraneh Vargha-Khadem of the Institute of Child Health in London.55
Some of the first linguists to study the affected family members believed their problem was specific to grammar but Vargha-Khadem has shown that it is considerably wider. Affected members have trouble in articulation, and the muscles of their lower face, particularly the upper lip, are relatively immobile.
It could be argued that their defect stemmed from some general malfunction in the brain, which was not specific to language. But the IQ scores of the affected members, though low, fell in a range (59 to 91) that overlapped with that of the unaffected members (84 to 119).56 The core deficit, Vargha-Khadem concluded, is “one that affects the rapid and precise coordination of orofacial [mouth and face] movements, including those required for the sequential articulation of speech sounds.”57
The affected members of the KE family, as it is known, have each inherited a single defective gene from their grandmother. They provide the results of an experiment that no one would even contemplate doing in humans, but which nature has performed nonetheless—what happens if you disable a critical speech gene? And the one disabled in the KE family seems to operate at such a sophisticated level that it looks as if it were one of the last genes to be put in place as the faculty of language was perfected.
In 1998 a team of geneticists at Oxford University in England set out to identify the defective gene by analyzing the genome of KE family members. Their method was to look for segments of DNA that the affected members shared and the unaffected lacked. The Oxford team soon narrowed the cause of the problem to a region on chromosome 7, the seventh of the 23 pairs of chromosomes in which the human genome is packaged. Within this region lay more than 70 genes, and it seemed that it would take several years to study each gene and see which one was responsible. But Hurst then turned up a new patient with the same rare set of symptoms. The patient, a boy, had a break in his chromosome 7 that disrupted one of the genes in the section the Oxford team was studying. It was an easy task to identify which of the new patient’s genes had been broken. It was a gene known as forkhead box P2, or FOXP2 for short.58
The Oxford geneticists, Cecilia Lai, Simon Fisher and Anthony Monaco, then analyzed all 267,000 DNA units in the FOXP2 genes of the KE family members. In all the affected members, and in none of the normal members, just one of these letters was changed fromaGto an A (the four different kinds of chemical units in DNA are known for short as A, T, G and C). The switch to an A at this site in the gene meant that in the protein molecule specified by the gene, a unit that should have been an arginine was changed to a histidine (proteins are made up of 20 different kinds of units, known as amino acids, of which arginine, and histidine are two).59
How could a single mutation in a gene cause such a wide range of effects? The FOX family of genes makes agents known as transcription factors, which operate at a high level of the cell’s control system. The agents bind to DNA and in doing so control the activity, or transcription, of many other genes. FOXP2 is active during fetal development in specific parts of the brain, and the protein transcription factor it makes probably helps wire up these brain regions correctly for language. Brain scans of affected KE family members seemed normal at first glance but a more sophisticated type of scan has shown they have considerably fewer neurons than usual in Broca’s area, one of the two brain regions known to be involved in language, and more neurons than usual in the other region, known as Wernicke’s area.60
FOXP2 is an ancient gene, and even mice possess a version of it. If the human version of the gene is intimately involved in the language faculty, then the gene would be expected to have changed in some significant way in the human lineage. Svante P
ääbo and colleagues at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany, analyzed the sequence of the FOXP2 gene in mice, in the great apes, and in people from the major continents. Some genes change quite rapidly over evolutionary time but FOXP2, they found, is highly conserved. Chimpanzees and gorillas carry the identical version of the gene, which must be the same as that possessed by the joint ancestor of chimps and humans who lived 5 million years ago. That version makes a protein that differs in only one of its 715 units from the version carried by mice, which shared a common ancestor with humans 70 million years ago. This means that from 70 million to 5 million years ago, a span of 65 million years, the FOXP2 protein underwent only a single change.
But its evolution suddenly accelerated in the human lineage after the human and chimp lineages diverged. The human version of the FOXP2 protein differs in two units from that of chimps, suggesting it was subject to some strong selection pressure such as must have accompanied the evolution of language.
All humans have essentially the same version of FOXP2, the sign of a gene so important that it has swept through the population and become universal. By analyzing the variations in the FOXP2 genes possessed by people around the world, Pääbo was able to fix a date, though rather roughly, for the time that all humans acquired the latest upgrade of the FOXP2 gene. It was fairly recently in human evolution, and certainly sometime within the last 200,000 years, he concluded.61
Language is such a complex faculty that it must be mediated by a large number of genes and have developed in several stages. Given the observation that the KE family’s deficit seems to be in the power of fluent, articulate speech, Pääbo thinks FOXP2 may have been one of the last genes recruited to the language function, perhaps the final step in the development of modern human speech. “Maybe it made the last perfection of language, made it totally modern,” he says.62