Book Read Free

The Book of Memory

Page 26

by Mary Carruthers


  gible language.

  Learning an alphabet is a part of grammar. This is also the point at which

  one lays down one’s fundamental mnemonic apparatus. Mnemonic writers

  like Thomas Bradwardine and John of Garland assume their students have

  certain sets of images already in their memories – in addition to alphabets

  and numbers, these include the Zodiac and the ‘‘characteristics of animals,’’

  voces animantium, a collection of attributes that derives from the various

  versions of the Bestiary, a work that itself goes back to Alexandrine Greece.

  Ordered lists of this sort, I propose, were deliberately memorized in order

  to serve as potential mnemonic heuristics, the seats (sedes) into which one

  could place the variety of diverse material one would acquire in one’s

  education and reading.

  Morgan Library MS. M 832, a manuscript written and painted in the last

  half of the twelfth century at the monastery of Go¨ttweig in Austria,

  contains in its first folio the letters of five alphabets – Hebrew, Greek,

  Latin, Scythian, and Runic, taken from Hrabanus Maurus ‘‘De inventione

  linguarum.’’ The rest of the manuscript is a Bestiary. 110 In light of the

  mnemonic role of alphabets and of the voces animantium, finding the two

  sorts of material together in a single manuscript makes some practical

  pedagogical sense, for such a manuscript would have served the monks

  by providing them with material to help them form sets of memory-places

  (indeed, it is hard otherwise to give a practical reason for twelfth-century

  Austrian monks to learn such alphabets as Scythian or Runic).

  There is evidence of scholars using both imaginary and real alphabetical

  sets later in their careers. Robert Grosseteste’s scheme of referencing

  symbols is derived eclectically from various alphabets, and many of his

  notae are imaginary. The Bolognese rhetoric master, Boncompagno da

  Signa, lists in his Rhetorica novissima (1235) a variety of signs and symbols

  that are useful as artificial aids to natural memory, including deposita

  alphabeta, and describes as well how he has used an imaginary alphabet

  Elementary memory design

  139

  as a memory code. By this means, he claims, ‘‘within thirty days I have

  memorized the names of five hundred students. And I also affirm, which

  will seem more remarkable, that in the sight of all I have named every one

  by his own name, not omitting his surname and where he was from:

  wherefore, together and singly, they were overcome with admiration.’’111

  Boncompagno’s feat may appear pallid in comparison with some of

  the feats of memory described by other scholars, but my concern is not

  with his talent but with his method – using an imaginary alphabet.

  Bernhard Bischoff describes some manuscripts from the ninth and eleventh

  centuries in which the names of the months and of numbers in Greek,

  Hebrew, and Coptic are given in parallel columns. Such lists, he says,

  ‘ worked like magnets’ in attracting names from other languages as well,

  but ‘ I think . . . these lists and enumerations in general cannot be regarded as

  the result of, or as an attempt at genuine language study. They might rather

  be regarded as a symptom of a naive curiosity which manifests itself also in

  the collecting – it is a kind of collecting – of foreign and strange alphabets

  which can be observed in manuscripts from the eighth century on and

  continued to post-medieval times,’’ collections which included, indiscrim-

  inately, both real and invented alphabets.112 Such a long life would suggest

  that readers found utility in these lists (after all, curiositas or the useless

  collecting of knowledge was regarded as sinful); they provided collections of

  notae that might be useful in schemes for memory coding, and supple-

  mented, as a reader might wish, those with which he was already familiar.

  Writing and memory

  It was Isidore of Seville, that chief conduit of ancient pedagogy to the

  medieval West, who defined the function of written letters in terms of their

  memorial utility. Alphabets are a kind of notae, and these written letters

  were invented ‘‘in order to remember things. For lest they fly into oblivion,

  they are bound in letters. For so great is the variety of things that all cannot

  be learned by hearing, nor contained only in memory.’’113 Writing is a

  servant to memory, a book its extension, and, like the memory itself,

  written letters call up the words of those that are no longer present. Two

  things are worth emphasizing in Isidore’s definition. First, the primacy of

  memory over writing, which one encounters as well in a text like Plato’s

  Phaedrus, is still present here in Isidore. Secondly, writing and memorizing

  are regarded by Isidore still as essentially the same process: writing is an

  activity of remembering, as remembering is writing on the tables of the

  mind. And all textual notae, such as marks of punctuation, or editorial signs

  140

  The Book of Memory

  and the shorthand symbols and abbreviations known properly as notaria,

  are of the same class as letters. Isidore defines a nota as ‘‘figura propria in

  litterae modum posita,’’ ‘‘a distinctive shape used in the manner of written

  letters.’’ Isidore’s understanding of a note, or for that matter of a letter, is

  not the same as our own, and this philological fact must be considered

  carefully. In modern linguistic theory, graphemes (‘‘letters’’) represent one

  or more phonemes, the units of meaningful sound that convey sense in any

  language; thus philosophical issues of adequacy and truth of representation

  can meaningfully apply to them. But a mnemonic cue does not represent

  anything necessarily. Its effectiveness resides entirely in its satisfactory

  performance as a mental marker, a reminder. Once it has successfully

  cued the matter it marks, its function is completed. For mnemonic cues,

  as indeed for tools generally, their ‘‘adequacy’’ is in respect of a task rather

  than an idea or theory. A screwdriver is adequate to its task when it drives a

  screw successfully. This is a fundamental definitional distinction, that must

  always be taken into account in medieval discussions of the nature of

  litterae, especially those contexts, chiefly pre-scholastic, in which the writer

  claims that written letters are ‘‘like’’ the notae of recollective investigation.

  By defining the essential utility of written letters as memorial in nature –

  writing is to keep us from forgetting – Isidore gives a remarkably succinct

  statement of a principle tenet of pre-modern pedagogy. A student in

  ancient schools (and these earliest accomplishments were the work of the

  nursery, according to Quintilian) first learned his letters, beginning with

  their names even before their forms, though Quintilian prefers, he says,

  that children learn the two together, endorsing especially the use of ivory

  alphabet-blocks for children to play with. In a custom deriving from Greek

  education, Latin letters were learned from A to X and then backwards from

  X to A, and then in pairs, such as AX, BV, CT, DS, and so on. Finally,


  breaking the order entirely, they were taught in all various combinations.114

  Quintilian rightly stresses that such a method teaches children to learn

  letters individually, not just in rote order. 115 But the exercise also induces a

  facility for calculating with the alphabet, that is moving it around quickly

  and surely in all sorts of both common and odd combinations. Such facility

  is very useful for the kind of mnemonic shuffling necessary for composi-

  tion, and would create an alphabetical heuristic as flexible as a numerical

  one. As Quintilian says, ‘‘the elements of reading and writing are entirely a

  matter of memoria’’; 116 he meant not only developing memorized content

  but, of equal importance, the capability for secure, rapid, and capacious

  system-building which characterizes trained memoria. ‘‘Memory . . . is very

  necessary to the orator; there is nothing like practice for nourishing and

  Elementary memory design

  141

  strengthening it, and, since the age-group of which we are now speaking

  cannot as yet produce anything on its own, it is almost the only faculty

  which the teacher’s attention can help develop.’’117

  After individual letters, instruction proceeded to syllables (ba-, be-, bo-,

  bi-, bu-, etc.) and then to words; all this time, writing was also taught, the

  student doing exercises on his wax tablet to complement the mental

  exercises performed on the tablet of his memory. Reciting was part of the

  reading–writing process from the very beginning. Quintilian recommends

  that students learn reading, recitation, and writing from material that

  contains sound moral advice, 118 the sayings (dicta) of famous men and

  lines from the poets. As Marrou says, ‘‘A la lecture et à l’ećriture est

  intimement associeé la rećitation: l’enfant apprend par coeur les petits

  textes sur lesquels il s’est exerce´, à la fois pour se former et meubler sa

  me´moire.’’119 ‘‘To form one’s character and furnish one’s memory’’ – they

  were the same goal in educational practice and philosophy from antiquity

  throughout the Middle Ages (and indeed well into the twentieth century).

  Reading required careful preparation, including learning the proper use of

  punctuation. Marrou mentions traces of such student-originated prepara-

  tion still observable on papyri (which were written without any sort of

  punctuation, even word divisions), where strokes have been made, by

  students not the scribe, to separate the words and lines, and to cut words

  up into syllables for scansion.120 Cola, commata, and periodi served a dual

  purpose; they marked the sense- and pause-divisions, and they also cut the

  text into the brief segments that could be memorized as a single unit.

  Quintilian says that a period should never be too long to be recited or read

  in a single breath. It also must not be ‘ too long to be retained in the memory’

  as a single, intelligible unit.121 A period contains at least two cola, Quintilian

  says, units that are not self-standing but are yet ‘‘rhythmically complete,’

  sensus numeris conclusus. 122 The use of rhythm to define units of prose is also

  mnemonically valuable, of course, for rhythm serves the synaesthetic require-

  ments of rich mnemonics. A basic memorial unit is the colon, a short

  segment of text that is coherent (it is, says Isidore, a sententia – ‘ sentence,’

  in Middle English), and is marked by a medial point in many manuscripts. 123

  Notae also included short-hand marks and abbreviations. Systems of

  such marks, numbering many hundreds, were taught as the notataria to

  ancient and medieval notaries and lawyers; students were taught the com-

  plex system of Latin abbreviations that we encounter in medieval manu-

  scripts. Thomas Bradwardine concludes his treatise on artificial memory

  (completed soon after 1333) by saying that ‘‘one who has learned the notary

  art will attain the highest perfection’’ in the craft of memory.

  142

  The Book of Memory

  John of Salisbury’s Metalogicon, written some fifty years after Hugh of

  St. Victor’s Chronicle Preface, is particularly interesting for the light it

  sheds on what was thought to be the link between memory and notaria.

  Like Hugh, John of Salisbury characterizes memory as ‘ the mind’s treasure-

  chest, a sure and reliable place of safe-deposit for perceptions.’ 124 Memory is

  a natural gift, but, like all natural abilities, must be cultivated and trained

  (here John echoes both Ad Herennium and De oratore).

  John of Salisbury especially laments the decline of training in notaria.

  He is discussing punctuation, and evidently thinking of what Isidore called

  the notae sententiarum, the editorial marks devised in antiquity for textual

  scholarship. These little notes, which ‘‘indicate the mode of what is written

  and show [what] is clear or obscure, certain or doubtful, and so on’’ were

  powerful tools, ‘‘highly effective for both comprehension and retention [my

  italics].’’ They are to reading and memory what notes are to the chant.

  That such great import has existed in such tiny notations should not seem strange,

  for [musically knowledgeable] singers likewise indicate by a few graphic symbols

  numerous variations in the acuteness and gravity of tones. For which reason such

  characters are appropriately known as ‘‘the keys of music.’’ If, however, the little

  notations we spoke of above gave access to such [a powerful key to knowledge],

  I am surprised that our forefathers, who were so learned, were not aware of this, or

  that the keys to so much knowledge were lost. 125

  Two things are being spoken of at once here. First of all, he speaks about

  the particular system of editorial notae (obelisk, apostrophe, etc.) described

  by Isidore of Seville, which John claims is no longer taught, and the secret

  of which has been lost. John of Salisbury also recognizes the general

  importance of all kinds of notae for memory training, for he assumes that

  these strange marks, like all the other notae, are memory aids, necessary for

  both retention and comprehension. And though the knowledge of some

  systems of ancient notae and memory coding are now obscure, still gram-

  mar, including memory, must be studied at least from the books we do

  have. For ‘‘if all the books of the grammarians are not available, it is still

  very helpful, for the interpretation of what we read, to have fixed in our

  memory even this fragmentary survey’’ (the Metalogicon itself).126

  That the ars notaria was associated specifically with memory training is

  also suggested by a twelfth-century gloss on the Ad Herennium, the so-

  called ‘‘Alanus’’ gloss, which may be the work of Alan of Lille, although no-

  one can say certainly.127 Commenting on the passage in Book III, in which

  Tullius cautions against those Greek works which offer lists of pre-formed

  imagines verborum, because they are both inadequate in scope and less

  effective for memory than images one makes oneself, the glossator writes

  Elementary memory design

  143

  that the author ‘‘did not have the notarial art in mind.’’128 He clearly knows,
r />   as did John of Salisbury, that notae are important for memory, and

  recognizes in his comment that the accumulation of notae that one learned

  in the advanced studies of this art helped to perfect the training of memory.

  Indices and chains

  Memorial notae were commonly used for concording schemes. Of these,

  the prototype is an alphabetical heuristic, which has left many traces in the

  organization of written texts. The existing account of it, which describes its

  use with a degree of detail comparable to what Hugh’s Preface tells us

  about the number grid, is very late (fifteenth-century). Still, what it

  describes is borne out by the actual design of medieval concordances,

  distinctio collections, and other preachers’ tools. And we also have

  Aristotle’s succinct account of an alphabetical memorial system in De

  memoria, Quintilian’s reference to key-words to aid recollection, and

  certain other evidence which we will look at soon. This alphabetical system

  produces what is essentially a catena, in which a key-word or phrase acts as

  the hook for several bits of stored material, the indexing words themselves

  being stored alphabetically. The monastic practice so well described by Dom

  Leclercq, ‘ whereby the verbal echoes [of Scripture] so excite the memory

  that a mere allusion will spontaneously evoke whole quotations, ’ 129 is a

  version of this type of memorial organization.

  The description occurs in the testamentary letter by Peter of Ravenna

  which I cited earlier in this discussion; it prefaces the memory rules which

  form the body of his book, Fenix. 130 As is the custom of such advertise-

  ments, the language is self-congratulatory and inflated, but the method

  itself is credible, and borne out by other evidence. Peter says he has

  ‘‘placed’’ 20,000 legal extracts, 1,000 texts from Ovid, 200 from Cicero,

  300 sayings of the philosophers, the greater part of Valerius Maximus,

  7,000 texts from Scripture, and other chunks of his learning on 19 letters of

  the Roman alphabet. If he searches the letter A, for example, he is able to

  produce immediately texts and examples from learned sources on a variety

  of subjects, ‘‘de alimentis, de alienatione, de absentia, de arbitris, de

  appellationibus, et de similibus quae jure nostro habentur incipientibus

 

‹ Prev