Book Read Free

The Book of Memory

Page 28

by Mary Carruthers


  especially since all the numbers of the book- or chapter-divisions are given

  in order (as ‘‘Augustinus: ep. 9. 11. 15. 20. 38. 50. 57; De civ. dei li. 10, 11, 12,

  13,’’ etc.).147 Why carefully order the one, but not the other, if one were

  organizing separate slips of parchment? That the design extends as far as

  authors, and then skips, as it were, to chapter numbers (the order in which

  a particular work is recalled), suggests that the citations are given in the

  order in which Grosseteste had stored them in memory, and not only from

  a physically manipulated source. 148

  So why was the subject concordance written down (though incom-

  pletely)? Presumably to be of use to other scholars, as indeed it proved

  for a long time to be. Grosseteste’s tabula amounts to a florilegium without

  the texts themselves being written out. A note in the manuscript tells us

  that Adam Marsh extended his master’s signs and classifications; indeed,

  space is left under some entries in the Lyons manuscript, presumably for

  additional entries. This concern for expansion suggests that Grosseteste’s

  system became public, presumably when his library came to the Oxford

  Franciscans. The index guides a student with access to this particular

  library to making his own index by memorizing those passages to which

  the notae lead him.

  150

  The Book of Memory

  Another interesting use of alphabetical organization is the second part of

  Book II of Albertus Magnus’s treatise, Mineralia. 149 Here the chapter

  headings state the nature of the organizing principle: ‘‘De lapidis pretiosis

  incipientibus ab A,’’ ‘‘Concerning precious stones beginning with A,’’

  and so on through Z. This would seem to reflect the way Albertus had

  organized his material mentally. Especially interesting is the fact that

  the names are not in absolute alphabetical order under each letter; so

  under B there are Balagius, Borax, Beryllus; under C, Carbunculus,

  Chalcedonis, Calcaphanos, Ceraurum, Celidonius, Celontes, Cegolites.

  Peter of Ravenna’s lists also are not absolutely alphabetical; this suggests

  someone arranging material mentally rather than alphabetizing written-

  out words with the aid of slips, simply because it is easier mentally to

  organize material using a few letters at a time than to alphabetize fully a

  number of long words. The latter is better done when one can manipulate

  written words physically, as anyone who has had to prepare an index will

  recognize. His method suggests strongly that Albertus is using a mental

  alphabetical heuristic, which he has simply transposed to a written docu-

  ment, writing on the page as he had written in memory before.

  One alphabetizing principle that seems very odd and useless to us is

  employed in the index to a late thirteenth-century manuscript of

  Sentences commentary by the Dominican friar Richard Fishacre, who

  died in 1248 (Oxford, New College MS. 112).150 The indexing words are

  arranged not according to initial letter but by their vowel combinations;

  in other words, by syllables rather than by letters. An explanation of the

  system is given (fo. 322):

  this preceding index is arranged according to the order of the vowels in the

  alphabet, and according to the manner of their combinations [with consonants];

  if you wish therefore promptly to find those things which are contained in the

  preceding book, take the subject concerning which the passage is made (princi-

  pally) or should be made, and regard the vowel of its syllable, or of each of its

  syllables if it should be a two-syllable word; and having recourse to the index you

  should find the vowel or vowels written in the margin and ordered as previously

  said. And opposite the word you will find written the folio number, the page

  number, and even the line number in which you will be able to find what you seek;

  knowing this also, that ‘‘a’’ designates the first column [of the book opening], ‘‘b’’

  the second, ‘‘c’’ the third, and ‘‘d’’ the fourth.

  Accordingly, the index begins with monosyllables in ‘‘a’’ (‘‘pax,’’ ‘‘laus,’’

  ‘‘pars’’); then bisyllables having ‘‘a’’ in each syllable (‘‘adam’’); then ‘‘a’’ plus

  ‘‘e’’; ‘‘a’’ plus ‘‘i’’; ‘‘a’’ plus ‘‘o’’; ‘‘a’’ plus ‘‘u’’; and so on through all five

  vowels singly and in combination. Only the first two syllables of

  Elementary memory design

  151

  multisyllabic words are considered for indexing purposes. This produces a

  list in which locutio and pondus both precede locus, pax precedes amor, and

  terra precedes spera, for the initial consonants are not considered in the

  scheme.

  Fishacre’s indexer belonged to a culture that still learned to read Latin by

  recognizing syllables. They were the basic units from which words were

  thought to be built. Word-recognition and retention was basically a matter

  of syllable-recognition. Thomas Bradwardine’s advice for word-memory is

  to make an image for each of the five vowels, and then for each possible

  combination of one consonant plus vowel. Remembering a whole word

  consists of stringing together the various images one has pre-formed for its

  syllables. The Fishacre index is based upon a similar understanding of how

  words are recalled, simplified to a string of no more than two. Why the

  compiler virtually disregarded the order of the consonants accompanying

  the vowels is mysterious. Evidently, in his mental arrangements, all the

  possible ‘‘a’’ syllables were grouped together, as Albertus Magnus grouped

  things by their initial letter. A scheme that groups by syllables is not

  inherently less useful as a mental heuristic than one which groups by initial

  letter. There were nineteen letters in the Roman alphabet, twenty-one if

  one includes ‘‘x’’ and ‘‘z,’’ but these were rarely used as initial consonants.

  But the Fishacre index allowed forty-five groups altogether, so that there

  are actually many fewer items within each cell of this grid than there are in

  initial-letter schemes. It should be noted, however, that no other surviving

  index uses this heuristic.

  A mental library of dictiones and a library of physical books used the

  same heuristics. Having already discussed at length the enduring image of

  the trained memory as a library, I will conclude this chapter with a brief

  reflection on their parallel cataloguing systems. The cupboard-like press in

  which medieval books were kept could be called an armarium, ‘‘cupboard,’’

  or a columna, ‘‘column,’’ the word used in a library catalogue from 1400. 151

  The books in these medieval arcae or armaria were labeled according to

  schemes of letters and numbers, sometimes used separately, sometimes in

  tandem. The sources we have for this information are library catalogues

  and sometimes the books themselves, mostly from the twelfth century and

  later. Typically, each book-press was assigned a letter, and each shelf

  (gradus) in it a number, starting from the bottom shelf to allow for

  expansion. 152 Sometimes another, subsidiary, nu
mber was assigned to

  each volume to indicate its place on its gradus. Often also the lettered

  bookcase carried a legend indicating the subject of the contents therein.

  Alphabetizing as a heuristic scheme for libraries dates at least to the

  152

  The Book of Memory

  Alexandrine Library. 153 The lay-out of the library and the design of the

  scholars’ memories imitate one another. Some of the best evidence for the

  similarity medieval scholars perceived between what is read and written in

  memory and in books is the way in which heuristic schemes taught for

  organizing one’s memorial arcae were also used, when collections became

  large enough to need them, to organize the manuscript codices in their

  wooden arcae.

  C H A P T E R 4

  The arts of memory

  The mnemonic techniques that I have described so far are basic, their

  elementary nature attested by their ubiquity. Divisio and compositio are

  processes required by the physiology of memory itself, at least as the pre-

  modern world understood it. But it is important to keep in mind that in

  considering medieval memoria we are dealing with more than just a set of

  techniques or a descriptive psychology, yet also a more specifically realized

  value than ghostly ideas (like ‘‘the Gothic’’) which modern students have

  attached to some philosophy or other, or to a metahistorical mentalite´.

  From antiquity, memoria was fully institutionalized in education, and like

  all vital practices it was adapted continuously to circumstances of history.

  Memoria unites written with oral transmission, eye with ear, and helps to

  account for the highly mixed oral–literate nature of medieval cultures,

  which many historians of the subject have remarked. Yet it is also clear that

  the later Middle Ages, from the eleventh century onward, was a far more

  bookish culture than the earlier medieval centuries had been. Memoria was

  adapted to that change, without – as a set of practices – losing its central

  place in medieval ethical life. In this chapter I will focus on this change by

  considering the revival in the thirteenth century of the Herennian architec-

  tural mnemonic which I described in Chapter 2.

  The medieval history of the particular mnemonic practice expounded in

  Rhetorica ad Herennium and espoused also by Cicero in De oratore is briefly

  summarized. Even by the time of Quintilian (first century AD), the method

  of projecting images into architectural places and making theatrical scenes

  of them, though still known, was considered cumbersome and gimmicky.

  Mnemonic practices continued to be cultivated – Quintilian enumerates a

  number of them in Book XI of Institutio oratoria – but the elaborate image-

  games and exercises to strengthen memoria verborum are not repeated

  there. Instead, Quintilian stresses mnemonics for the disposition of one’s

  subject matters when speaking ex tempore, and using rebuses and puns to

  recall unfamiliar words or especially important items. This practical

  153

  154

  The Book of Memory

  attitude persists in the rhetorical teaching on memory of the fourth and

  fifth centuries. Indeed there is little evidence of anyone, ancient or medi-

  eval, systematically teaching the Rhetorica ad Herennium before the end of

  the eleventh century. However, in considering the medieval understanding

  of rhetorical craft, including memoria, a historian must clearly distinguish

  between those methods described and used in actual practice, and what is

  described in and disseminated solely through the two ancient rhetoric

  textbooks known in the Middle Ages as the ‘‘vetus rhetorica’’ (Cicero’s

  early work De inventione) and the ‘‘rhetorica nova’’ (the Rhetorica ad

  Herennium). The medieval titles refer not to the actual order of composi-

  tion of these works (for in fact their compositional order is the reverse,

  though they were made close in time to one another) but to the order in

  which they were incorporated into the medieval curriculum of rhetoric, for

  the Rhetorica ad Herennium was not much taught at Bologna, Oxford, or

  Paris before the thirteenth century. Its first widely used commentary, and

  thus the first evidence of its entering a broadly influential curriculum, is the

  Parisian ‘‘Alanus’’ commentary of the late twelfth century. Bolognese

  masters of the first part of the thirteenth century, such as Boncompagno

  da Signa, report it as little taught, though Boncompagno had clearly read it,

  for he parodies its medieval title in his own work of 1235, Rhetorica

  novissima. Herennian memoria plays no direct role in the development of

  medieval memory craft before the thirteenth century, and when it was

  revived in university teaching after 1250, it was within the context of an

  extraordinarily rich, diverse understanding of memoria, which had devel-

  oped in monastic traditions of ruminative meditation and the composition

  of prayer. Monastic memoria is more like what is now called ‘‘mindfulness’’

  than what many psychologists deem to be memory, a discipline of attentive

  recollection and concentrated reading of texts in the Bible (lectio divina),

  which took place during the daily office and in private meditation, and

  formed the core of monastic life. But it should not simply be translated as

  ‘‘mindfulness’’ because the activity of lectio divina assumed a searchable

  inventory of remembered materials far richer than any student might be

  expected to acquire today. These practices are analyzed historically and

  aesthetically in my study of The Craft of Thought, and additionally demon-

  strated in the twelfth-century works brought together in The Medieval

  Craft of Memory. When the Herennian architectural mnemonic described

  by Frances Yates and others was revived in the late medieval universities, it

  was within these well-established monastic practices. One cannot separate

  it out from them without seriously distorting late medieval and early

  modern method and understanding.1 Whether they acknowledged their

  The arts of memory

  155

  debt or not (and, being ‘‘modern,’’ most did not), the Renaissance writers

  on the art of memory wrote still from within a profoundly medieval

  context. 2

  In the late thirteenth century the architectural method enjoyed a revival,

  having been commended as the best method by both Albertus Magnus and

  Thomas Aquinas. 3 The revival of Tullius’ art of memory occurred in the

  context of the classicizing fervor of the early humanists, among whom, for

  the purposes of this history, we must include the friars chiefly responsible

  for the Aristotelian renaissance that brought back into scholarly circulation

  his De anima and its appendices, the Parva naturalia including De memoria

  et reminiscentia. Aristotle gave a philosophical explanation for and author-

  itative vindication of using arbitrary associations as a basic mnemonic

  tool. Both Thomas’s and Albertus’s commentaries on De memoria use

  the memory advice of the Ad Herennium as their prime example of the

  application of Aristotle’s general precepts concerning the associat
ive nature

  of recollective searching. In addition to the Aristotelians, the self-described

  Ciceronians of early humanist Italy played an important role in the revival

  of the Herennian mnemonic; indeed, some evidence points to the circle

  around Brunetto Latini as one agency of this revival. And it is the identi-

  fication of the Herennian mnemonic scheme with humanism that led to

  its dominance in the memory texts of the Renaissance, many of which

  emanate from a milieu that is Italian and also Dominican. To that story,

  however, I will return after examining more particularly medieval

  schemes. 4

  J O H N O F G A R L A N D ( C . 1 2 3 0 ) A N D T H E B E S T I A R Y

  The sources of the specific medievalness of the arts of memory I will discuss

  now are not attributable to handbooks and manuals, such as some

  (unknown) non-Ciceronian body of rhetorical precepts. They are related

  instead to manuscript painting conventions, the Bestiary, and various

  conventions of pictorial diagrams. In the early Middle Ages, memoria is

  discussed not in the context of how best to teach rhetoric, but rather

  in writings on meditation and prayer, in which a diagram-like picture

  (pictura) is created mentally, which serves as the site for a meditational

  collatio, the gathering into one place of the various strands of a meditated

  composition. A particularly illuminating and complex example utilizing

  the principles of medieval memorial picturing is Hugh of St. Victor’s

  treatise ‘‘On constructing Noah’s ark,’’ which I will discuss at length in

  Chapter 7. 5

  156

  The Book of Memory

  That text is not, however, an art of memory produced within the

  universities, the subject of the present chapter. In particular I will examine

  three academic texts which defined ‘‘arts of memory,’’ written by John of

  Garland, Albertus Magnus, and Thomas Bradwardine. They all incorpo-

  rate a medieval understanding of the precepts of memoria that has wrought

  a thorough sea-change upon the Herennian mnemonic. I have chosen to

  discuss them out of chronological order because neither Bradwardine nor

  John of Garland show any close affinity to the Ad Herennium, but Albertus

  does – therefore, John of Garland and Bradwardine are good examples of

 

‹ Prev