The Gap

Home > Other > The Gap > Page 40
The Gap Page 40

by Thomas Suddendorf


  Grun, R., et al. (2005). U-series and ESR analyses of bones and teeth relating to the human burials from Skhul. Journal of Human Evolution, 49, 316–334.

  Guinet, C., & Bouvier, J. (1995). Development of intentional stranding hunting techniques in killer whale (Ornicus orca) calves at Crozet Archipelago. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 73, 27–33.

  Gupta, A. S., et al. (2010). Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience. Neuron, 65, 695–705.

  Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2007). Longevity among hunter-gatherers: A cross-cultural examination. Population and Development Review, 33, 321–365.

  Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316, 998–1002.

  Haile-Selassie, Y. (2001). Late Miocene hominids from the Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature, 412, 178–181.

  Haile-Selassie, Y., et al. (2012). A new hominin foot from Ethiopia shows multiple Pliocene bipedal adaptations. Nature, 483, 565–569.

  Halford, G. S., et al. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–864.

  Halford, G. S., et al. (2007). Separating cognitive capacity from knowledge: A new hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 236–242.

  Hamann, K., et al. (2011). Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476, 328–331.

  Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.

  Hamlin, J. K., et al. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.

  Hammer, M. F., et al. (2011). Genetic evidence for archaic admixture in Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 15123–15128.

  Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571–581.

  Hare, B., et al. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771–785.

  Hare, B., et al. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151.

  Hare, B., et al. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101, 495–514.

  Hare, B., et al. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: Evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83, 573–585.

  Harré, N. (2011). Psychology for a better world. Auckland: Department of Psychology.

  Harman, O. S. (2010). The price of altruism: George Price and the search for the origins of kindness. New York: Norton.

  Harris, P. L., et al. (1996). Children’s use of counterfactual thinking in causal reasoning. Cognition, 61, 233–259.

  Hart, D., & Sussman, R. W. (2005). Man the hunted: Primates, predators, and human evolution. Boulder: Westview Press.

  Haun, D. B. M., & Call, J. (2008). Imitation recognition in great apes. Current Biology, 18, R288-R290.

  Hauser, M. D. (1996). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Hauser, M. D., & Marler, P. (1993). Food associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Ecology, 4, 206–212.

  Hauser, M. D., et al. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

  Hawkes, K. (2003). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. American Journal of Human Biology, 15, 380–400.

  Hayes, C. (1951). The ape in our house. New York: Harper.

  Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 450–459.

  Hazlitt, W. (1805). Essay on the principles of human action and some remarks on the systems of Hartley and Helvetius. London: J. Johnson.

  Heinrich, B. (1995). An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens (Corvus corax). Auk, 112, 994–1003.

  Henrich, J., et al. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 1767–1770.

  Herculano-Houzel, S. (2009). The human brain in numbers: A linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 1–11.

  Herman, L. (2002). Vocal, social and self-imitation by bottlenosed dolphins. In Dautenhahn, K., & Nehaniv, C. (Eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. (pp. 63–108). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  Herman, L. M., et al. (1993). Representational and conceptual skills of dolphins. In H. L. Roitblat, L. M. Herman, & P. E. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and communication: Comparative perspectives (pp. 403–442). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  Herrmann, E., et al. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 1360–1366.

  Herschel, J. (1830). A preliminary discourse on the study of natural philosophy. London: Longman et al.

  Hewstone, M., et al. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 575–604.

  Heyes, C. M. (1994). Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour, 47, 909–919.

  Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 101–134.

  Hill, A., et al. (2011). Inferential reasoning by exclusion in great apes, lesser apes, and spider monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 91–103.

  Hill, K., et al. (2009). The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 187–200.

  Hoffer, E. (1973). Reflections on the human condition. New York: Harper & Row.

  Hofreiter, M., et al. (2010). Vertebrate DNA in fecal samples from bonobos and gorillas: Evidence for meat consumption or artefact? PLOS One, 5, e9419.

  Holdaway, R. N., & Jacomb, C. (2000). Rapid extinction of the moas (Aves: dinorinthiformes): Model, test, and implications. Science, 287, 2250–2254.

  Holden, C. (2005). Time’s up on time travel. Science, 308, 1110.

  Holloway, R. L. (2008). The human brain evolving: A personal retrospective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 1–19.

  Holmes, R. (2008). The age of wonder: How the romantic generation discovered the beauty and terror of science. London: Harper Press.

  Holzhaider, J. C., et al. (2010). The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour, 147, 553–586.

  Hoppitt, W. J. E., et al. (2008). Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23, 486–493.

  Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164–181.

  Horner, V., et al. (2010). Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLOS One, 5, e10625.

  Horner, V., et al. (2011). Spontaneous prosocial choice by chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 13847–13851.

  Hudson, J. A. (2006). The development of future time concepts through mother-child conversation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 70–95.

  Huffman, M. A. (1997). Current evidence for self-medication in primates: A multidisciplinary perspective. In Yearbook of Physical Anthropology (Vol. 40, pp. 1–30). Wilmington, DE: Wiley-Liss.

  Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–313). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Hunt, G., & Gray, R. (2003). Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 270, 867–874.

  Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia, 28, 517–527.

  Huxley, A. (1956). Adonis and the alphabet. London: Chatto & Windus.

  Hyatt, C. W. (1998). Responses of gibbons (Hylobates lar) to their mirror images. American Journal of Primatology, 45, 307–311.

  Inoue, S., & Matsuzawa, T. (2007). W
orking memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 17, R1004-R1005.

  Isanski, B., & West, C. (2010). The body of knowledge: Understanding embodied cognition. Observer, 23, 13–18.

  Jackendoff, R., & Pinker, S. (2005). The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language. Cognition, 97, 211–225.

  James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. London: Macmillan.

  Jantz, R. L. (2001). Cranial change in Americans: 1850–1975. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 46, 784–787.

  Jerison, H. J. (1973). The evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.

  Johanson, D. C. (2004). Lucy, thirty years later: An expanded view of Australopithecus afarensis. Journal of Anthropological Research, 60, 465–486.

  Johnson, A., & Redish, A. D. (2007). Neural ensembles in CA3 transiently encode paths forward of the animal at a decision point. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 12176–12189.

  Johnson, C. R., & McBrearty, S. (2010). 500,000 year old blades from the Kapthurin Formation, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution, 58, 193–200.

  Johnson, M. A. (April 18, 2005). The culture of Einstein: Achievements in science gave him a platform to address the world. NBC News. Retrieved from http://www.msnbc.msn.com/id/7406337/#.UD2tYUQuKHk

  Jones, B. W., & Nishiguchi, M. K. (2004). Counterillumination in the Hawaiian bobtail squid, Euprymna scolopes Berry (Mollusca: Cephalopoda). Marine Biology, 144, 1151–1155.

  Kaertner, J., et al. (2012). The development of mirror self-recognition in different sociocultural contexts. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 1–101.

  Kafka, F. (2009). A report for an academy. (I. Johnston, Trans.). Retrieved from http://records.viu.ca/~johnstoi/kafka/reportforacademy.htm

  Kaminski, J., et al. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “fast mapping.” Science, 5677, 1682–1683.

  Kaminski, J., et al. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224–234.

  Kana, R. K., et al. (2011). A systems level analysis of the mirror neuron hypothesis and imitation impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 894–902.

  Karlsson, M. P., & Frank, L. M. (2009). Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 913–918.

  Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behaviour of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islets. Primates, 6, 1–30.

  Kawai, N., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee. Nature, 403, 39–40.

  Keeley, L. H. (1996). War before civilization. London: Oxford University Press.

  Kivell, T. L., & Schmitt, D. (2009). Independent evolution of knuckle-walking in African apes shows that humans did not evolve from a knuckle-walking ancestor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 14241–14246.

  Klein, S. B., et al. (2002). Memory and temporal experience: The effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesic patient’s ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Social Cognition, 20, 353–379.

  Kohlberg, L. (1963). Development of children’s orientation toward a moral order. Vita Humana, 6, 11–33.

  Köhler, W. (1917/1925). The mentality of apes. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  Korsgaard, C. (2006). Morality and the distinctiveness of human action. In F. B. M. de Waal (Ed.), Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved (pp. 98–119). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

  Krachun, C., et al. (2009a). Can chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) discriminate appearance from reality? Cognition, 112, 435–450.

  Krachun, C., et al. (2009b). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science, 12, 521–535.

  Krause, J., et al. (2007a). The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals. Current Biology, 17, 1908–1912.

  Krause, J., et al. (2007b). Neanderthals in central Asia and Siberia. Nature, 449, 902–904.

  Krause, J., et al. (2010). The complete mitochondrial DNA genome of an unknown hominin from southern Siberia. Nature, 464, 894–897.

  Kuhlmeier, V. A., & Boysen, S. T. (2002). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) recognize spatial and object correspondences between a scale model and its referent. Psychological Science, 13, 60–63.

  Kundera, M. (1992). Immortality. (P. Kussi, Trans.). New York: HarperCollins. Lahdenpera, M., et al. (2004). Fitness benefits of prolonged post-reproductive lifespan in women. Nature, 428, 178–181.

  Lalueza-Fox, C., et al. (2007). A melanocortin 1 receptor allele suggests varying pigmentation among Neanderthals. Science, 318, 1453–1455.

  Lancaster, J. B., & Lancaster, C. S. (1983). Parental investment: The hominid adaptation. In D. J. Ortner (Ed.), How humans adapt: A biocultural odyssey. Washington: Smithsonian Institution.

  Langdon, J. H. (2006). Has an aquatic diet been necessary for hominin brain evolution and functional development? British Journal of Nutrition, 96, 7–17.

  Langley, M. C., et al. (2008). Behavioural complexity in Eurasian Neanderthal populations: A chronological examination of the archaeological evidence. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18, 289–307.

  Larick, R., & Ciochon, R. L. (1996). The African emergence and early Asian dispersals of the genus Homo. American Scientist, 84, 538–551.

  Lea, S. E. G. (2001). Anticipation and memory as criteria for special welfare consideration. Animal Welfare, 10, S195–208.

  Leakey, L. S. B., et al. (1964). A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai gorge. Nature, 202, 7–9.

  Leakey, M. D., & Hay, R. L. (1979). Pliocene footprints in the Laetolil beds at Laetoli, Northern Tanzania. Nature, 278, 317–323.

  Leakey, M. G., et al. (2001). New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature, 410, 433–440.

  Leaver, L. A., et al. (2007). Audience effects on food caching in grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis): Evidence for pilferage avoidance strategies. Animal Cognition, 10, 23–27.

  Lebel, C., et al. (2012). Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan. NeuroImage, 60, 340–352.

  Lepre, C. J., et al. (2011). An earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477, 82–85.

  Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation in infancy: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.

  Lethmate, J., & Dücker, G. (1973). Untersuchungen zum Selbsterkennen im Spiegel bei Orang-Utans und einigen anderen Affenarten [Investigations into self-recognition in orangutans and some other apes]. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 33, 248–269.

  Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2012). Tools from evolutionary biology shed new light on the diversification of languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 167–173.

  Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. Child Development, 75, 1821–1831.

  Lewis, M., et al. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156.

  Lieberman, P. (1991). Uniquely human: The evolution of speech, thought and selfless behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  Lindsay, W. L. (1880). Mind in the lower animals, in health and disease. New York: Appleton and Co.

  Linnaeus, C. (1758). Systema naturae (10th edition). Stockholm: Laurentii Sylvii.

  Liszkowski, U., et al. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7, 297–307.

  Liszkowski, U., et al. (2009). Prelinguistic infants, but not chimpanzees, communicate about absent entities. Psychological Science, 20, 654–660.

  Locke, J. L., & Bogin, B. (2006). Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 259–280.

  Loftus, E. F. (1992). When a lie becomes memory’s truth: Memory distortion after exposure to misinformation. Current Di
rections in Psychological Science, 1, 121–123.

  Lombard, M. (2012). Thinking through the Middle Stone Age of sub-Saharan Africa. Quaternary International, 270, 140–155.

  Lordkipanidze, D., et al. (2005). The earliest toothless hominin skull. Nature, 434, 717–718.

  Luna, B., et al. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75, 1357–1372.

  Lyn, H., et al. (2008). Precursors of morality in the use of the symbols “good” and “bad” in two bonobos (Pan paniscus) and a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Language & Communication, 28, 213–224.

  Lyn, H., et al. (2011). Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Language & Communication, 31, 63–74.

  Madsen, E. A., et al. (2007). Kinship and altruism: A cross-cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 339–359.

  Mahajan, N., et al. (2011). The evolution of intergroup bias: Perceptions and attitudes in rhesus macaques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 387–405.

  Martinez, I., et al. (2008). Human hyoid bones from the middle Pleistocene site of the Sima de los Huesos (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 118–124.

  Mäthger, L. M., et al. (2009). Do cephalopods communicate using polarized light reflections from their skin? Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 2133–2140.

  Matsuzawa, T. (2009). Symbolic representation of number in chimpanzees. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 92–98.

  Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development, 73, 969–982.

  McAuliffe, K. (2010, September). The incredible shrinking brain. Discover Magazine, 31, 54–59.

  McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence, 33, 337–346.

  McDougall, I., et al. (2005). Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature, 433, 733–736.

  McGuigan, F., & Salmon, K. (2004). The time to talk: The influence of the timing of adult-child talk on children’s event memory. Child Development, 75, 669–686.

 

‹ Prev