McHenry, H. M., & Coffing, K. (2000). Australopithecus to Homo: Transformations in body and mind. Annual Review of Anthropology, 29, 125–146.
McIlwain, D. (2003). Bypassing empathy: A Machiavellian theory of mind and sneaky power. In B. Repacholi & V. P. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind (pp. 39–66). New York: Psychology Press.
McPherron, S. P., et al. (2010). Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature, 466, 857–860.
Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–541.
Melis, A. P., et al. (2006a). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science, 311, 1297–1300.
Melis, A. P., et al. (2006b). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72, 275–286.
Melis, A. P., et al. (2008). Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Animal Behaviour, 76, 951–962.
Mellars, P. (2006). Going east: New genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science, 313, 796–800.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds and immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.
Mendes, N., & Huber, L. (2004). Object-permanence in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 103–112.
Mendes, N., et al. (2007). Raising the level: Orangutans use water as a tool. Biology Letters, 3, 453–455.
Menzel, E. (2005). Progress in the study of chimpanzee recall and episodic memory. In H. S. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition (pp. 188–224). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mercader, J., et al. (2007). 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 3043–3048.
Mesoudi, A., et al. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329-347.
Mikhail, J. (2007). Universal moral grammar: Theory, evidence and the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 143–152.
Miles, H. L., et al. (1996). Simon says: The development of imitation in an enculturated orangutan. In A. E. Russon, S.T. Parker, & K. A. Bard (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 278–299). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Miles, L. (1994). The cognitive foundations for references in a single orangutan. In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), ‘Language’ and intelligence in monkeys and apes (pp. 511–539). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 141–144.
Miller, G. F. (1998). How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution. Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 87–129.
Milot, E., et al. (2011). Evidence for evolution in response to natural selection in a contemporary human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 17040–17045.
Mischel, W., et al. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.
Mitchell, A., et al. (2009). Adaptive prediction of environmental changes by microorganisms. Nature, 460, 220–224.
Mitchell, R. W., & Anderson, J. R. (1993). Discrimination-learning of scratching, but failure to obtain imitation and self-recognition in a long-tailed macaque. Primates, 34, 301–309.
Moore, C. (2006). The development of common sense psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Moore, C. (2013). Homology through development of triadic interaction and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 59–66.
Morete, M. E., et al. (2003). A novel behavior observed in humpback whales on wintering grounds at Abrolhos Bank (Brazil). Marine Mammal Science, 19, 694–707.
Morgan, E. (1982). The aquatic ape: A theory of human evolution. London: Souvenir.
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006a). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 1038–1040.
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006b). How great apes perform on a modified trap-tube task. Animal Cognition, 9, 193–199.
Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2009). The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 304–309.
Mulcahy, N. J., & Suddendorf, T. (2011). An obedient orangutan (Pongo abelii) performs perfectly in peripheral object-choice tasks but fails the standard centrally presented versions. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 112–115.
Mulcahy, N. J., et al. (2005). Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 23–32.
Mulcahy, N. J., et al. (2013). Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii) understand connectivity in the skewered grape tool task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 109–113.
Murray, C. M., et al. (2007). New case of intragroup infanticide in the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 23–37.
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., et al. (2004). Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science, 7, 437–442.
Naqshbandi, M., & Roberts, W. A. (2006). Anticipation of future events in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and rats (Rattus norvegicus): Tests of the Bischof-Kohler hypothesis. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 345–357.
Neisser, U. (1997). The roots of self-knowledge. In J. G. Snodgrass & R. L. Thompson (Eds.), The Self Across Psychology (Vol. 818, pp. 19–33). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Neisser, U., et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.
Nelson, K. D., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486–511.
Nesse, R. M., & Berridge, K. C. (1997). Psychoactive drug use in evolutionary perspective. Science, 278, 63–66.
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565.
Nielsen, M., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Pretend play, mirror self-recognition and imitation: A longitudinal investigation through the second year. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 342–365.
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729–736.
Nielsen, M., & Widjojo, E. (2011). Failure to find over-imitation in captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus): Implications for our understanding of cross-generational information transfer. In J Håkansson (Ed.), Developmental Psychology (pp.153–167). New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Nielsen, M., et al. (2005). Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 8, 31–36.
Nielsen, M., et al. (2006). Mirror self-recognition beyond the face. Child Development, 77, 176–185.
Nimchinsky, E. A., et al. (1999). A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 5268–5273.
Noack, R. A. (2012). Solving the “human problem”: The frontal feedback model. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1043–1067.
Noad, M. J., et al. (2000). Cultural revolution in whale songs. Nature, 408, 537.
O’Connell, J. F., et al. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 461–485.
O’Neill, D. K., e
t al. (1992). Young children’s understanding of the role that sensory experiences play in knowledge acquisition. Child Development, 63, 474–490.
Oberauer, K., et al. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: Their correlation and their relation. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 61–65.
Oberauer, K., et al. (2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence? Intelligence, 36, 641–652.
Okuda, J., et al. (2003). Thinking of the future and past: The roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage, 19, 1369–1380.
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.
Ostrom, E. (2009). Beyond markets and states: Polycentric governance of complex economic systems. Nobel Prize Lectures. Retrieved from http://www.nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/economics/laureates/2009/ostrom-lecture.html
Osvath, M. (2009). Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Current Biology, 19, R190–R191.
Osvath, M., & Osvath, H. (2008). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674.
Oxnard, C., et al. (2010). Post-cranial skeletons of hypothyroid cretins show a similar anatomical mosaic as Homo floresiensis. PLOS One, 5, e13018.
Parker, C. E. (1974a). The antecedents of man the manipulator. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 493–500.
Parker, C. E. (1974b). Behavioral diversity in ten species of nonhuman primates. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 5, 930–937.
Parr, L. A. (2001). Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 4, 223–229.
Patterson, F. (1991). Self-awareness in the gorilla Koko. Gorilla, 14, 2–5.
Patterson, F., & Linden, E. (1981). The education of Koko. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Patterson, N., et al. (2006). Genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees. Nature, 441, 1103–1108.
Paukner, A., et al. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science, 325, 880–883.
Paus, T., et al. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: In vivo study. Science, 283, 1908–1911.
Penn, D. C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind.’ Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 362, 731–744.
Penn, D. C., et al. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 109–178.
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987). Acquisition of the same different concept by an African grey parrot. Animal Learning & Behavior, 15, 423–432.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind and Language, 15, 123–145.
Peterson, C. C., et al. (2000). Factors influencing the development of a theory of mind in blind children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 431–447.
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. London: Penguin.
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. London: Penguin.
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 8993–8999.
Pinker, S. (2011a). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined. New York: Penguin.
Pinker, S. (2011b). Representations and decision rules in the theory of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 35–37.
Plotnik, J. M., et al. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 17053–17057.
Posada, S., & Colell, M. (2007). Another gorilla recognizes himself in a mirror. American Journal of Primatology, 69, 576–583.
Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, 1–198.
Povinelli, D. J., et al. (1990). Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 203–210.
Povinelli, D. J., et al. (1992). Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: Evidence of empathy? Animal Behaviour, 43, 633–640.
Povinelli, D. J., et al. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence for a developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 1540–1554.
Povinelli, D. J., et al. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 12, 327–365.
Povinelli, D. J., et al. (2000). Toward a science of other minds: Escaping the argument by analogy. Cognitive Science, 24, 509–542.
Premack, D. (2007). Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 13861–13867.
Premack, D. (2012). Why humans are unique: Three theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 22–32.
Premack, D., & Premack, A. (1983). The mind of an ape. New York: Norton.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Preuss, T. M. (2000). What’s human about the human brain? In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1219–1234). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Preuss, T. M., et al. (1999). Distinctive compartmental organization of human primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 11601–11606.
Priel, B., & Deschonen, S. (1986). Self-recognition: A study of a population without mirrors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 237–250.
Prior, H., et al. (2008). Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of self-recognition. PLOS Biology, 6, 1642–1650.
Proffitt, D. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.
Pruetz, J. D., & Bertolani, P. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17, 412–417.
Radick, G. (2007). The simian tongue. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rafetseder, E.R., et al. (2010). Counterfactual reasoning: Developing a sense of “nearest possible world.” Child Development, 81, 376–389.
Ramirez Rozzi, F. V., & Bermudez De Castro, J. M. (2004). Surprisingly rapid growth in Neanderthals. Nature, 428, 936–939.
Ramirez Rozzi, F. V., et al. (2009). Cutmarked human remains bearing Neandertal features and modern human remains associated with the Aurignacian at Les Rois. Journal of Anthropological Sciences = Rivista di antropologia : JASS / Istituto italiano di antropologia, 87, 153–185.
Range, F., et al. (2009). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 340–345.
Ranlet, P. (2000). The British, the Indians, and smallpox: What actually happened at Fort Pitt in 1763? Pennsylvania History, 67, 427–441.
Read, D. W. (2008). Working memory: A cognitive limit to non-human primate recursive thinking prior to hominid evolution. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 676–714.
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (Eds.). (2003). Animal innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Reed, D. L., et al. (2007). Pair of lice lost or parasites regained: The evolutionary history of anthropoid primate lice. BMC Biology, 5:7.
Reich, D., et al. (2011). Denisova admixture and the first modern human dispersals into Southeast Asia and Oceania. American Jo
urnal of Human Genetics, 89, 516–528.
Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 5937–5942.
Rendell, L., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–324.
Rice, G. E., & Gainer, P. (1962). “Altruism” in the albino rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 123–125.
Ridley, M. (1997). The origins of virtue: Human instincts and the evolution of cooperation. New York: Viking.
Rizzolatti, G., et al. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.
Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–489.
Robson, S. L., & Wood, B. (2008). Hominin life history: Reconstruction and evolution. Journal of Anatomy, 212, 394–425.
Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (2011). Remember when? Science, 333, 47–48.
Roma, P. G., et al. (2006). Capuchin monkeys, inequity aversion, and the frustration effect. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 67–73.
Roma, P. G., et al. (2007). Mark tests for self-recognition in Capuchin monkeys (Cebus paella) trained to touch marks. American Journal of Primatology, 69, 989–1000.
Rosati, A. G., et al. (2007). The evolutionary origins of human patience: Temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology, 17, 1663–1668.
Roth, G., & Dicke, U. (2005). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 250–257.
Ruffman, T., et al. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.
Russell, B. (1954). Human society in ethics and politics. New York: Allan and Unwin.
Russell, B. (2009). The basic writings of Bertrand Russell. New York: Routledge.
Russon, A. E., & Galdikas, B. M. (1993). Imitation in free-ranging rehabilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 147–161.
Ruxton, G. D., & Wilkinson, D. M. (2011). Avoidance of overheating and selection for both hair loss and bipedality in hominins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 20965–20969.
The Gap Page 41