Dog Sense
Page 30
Interestingly, while dogs love to sniff other dogs, it seems that most dogs don’t much like being sniffed themselves. It is almost always the dog being sniffed who attempts to break off the interaction. Thus while dogs want to find out as much as they can about other dogs’ “odor signatures,” they seem reluctant to give away their own. (Young wolves share this reluctance, so this may be where the behavior originated.) It’s as if they see information about the dogs around them as the key to—well, something—since once the sniffing is finished the interaction usually ends. What that “something” is has yet to be resolved. If they’re successful in their encounters, dogs go home from walks with lots of information in their heads about what the other dogs in their neighborhood smell like. What they do with this information is currently unclear.
In fact, there is a great deal we don’t know about the kinds of information dogs can get by sniffing each other. The anal sac may signify more than individual identity; for example, for a wolf it might also indicate which pack it belongs to, if members of a pack share an odor. Some components of the wolf’s anal sacs may also vary according to gender and reproductive state. The same might be true for dogs, but at present we don’t know.
It’s remarkable that we know so little about the one activity, sniffing, that dogs like to do the most. Nothing better exemplifies how human-centered we can be when thinking about our domestic animals. Somehow we fail to grasp the “otherness” of much of what they experience. Of course, to dogs what something smells like is not “other”; it is, if anything, more important than what it looks like.
The dog’s fascination with odor must have originated way back in its evolutionary past. Scent is a major mode of communication for a wide variety of animals (it is humans who are the exception, not dogs). In particular, scent is a good way of transmitting information between animals that live far apart from one another. The early carnivores, the dog’s remote ancestors, are very unlikely to have lived in groups. They were almost certainly solitary, defending territories against other members of their own kind. The only groups would have been mothers and their dependent young, who would have stayed together for a few months at most before the young were old enough to disperse. Communication between adults would therefore have revolved around establishing and maintaining territorial boundaries. Apart from courtship and mating, face-to-face meetings would have been a rarity. Not only that, they would have been risky: Being well-armed with teeth and claws, carnivores try to avoid disputes that damage both parties, not just the loser. Finally, these animals were probably nocturnal, inhibiting visual communication. In the natural world, all these issues can be circumvented by using scent-marking as the primary mode of long-distance communication. A scent-mark designed for that purpose can last for days. Messages can be left for recipients to pick up at some undetermined moment in the future, obviating any necessity for actual meetings to take place. Contemporary dogs, who evolved from sociable animals and have become yet more sociable with domestication, may no longer need to scent-mark as frequently as they obviously think they do; but their wild ancestors must have found it very advantageous, and their legacy remains in our dogs’ everyday behavior.
Nowhere is this more obvious than in the dog’s apparent obsession with depositing small quantities of urine as scent-marks. Male dogs are renowned for their raised-leg urinations, but females also urine-mark routinely: Although they usually squat to urinate, many also use a “squat-raise” marking posture. It’s not entirely clear why females leave scent-marks; one clue may be found in the observation that bitches in Indian villages squat-raise around their denning sites. The male dogs in the same villages perform their characteristic “raised-leg urination” everywhere, but especially at the boundaries of their family-group territories. 11 Male wolves, particularly breeding males, also mark at territorial boundaries and along frequently used paths, presumably as a way of communicating with other packs nearby.
“Squat-raise” urine-marking
The domestic dog’s passion for “pee-mail” can therefore be traced back through its immediate ancestor, but this does not explain why pet dogs do it so enthusiastically today. Perhaps they would like to “own” the area where their owners take them for exercise; however, because they have to share this with other dogs and their access is time-limited by their owner, they get caught up in a vicious cycle. Every time they go out, they find that the scent-marks they left yesterday have been overmarked by other dogs. So they have to mark again to reestablish their claim to ownership, and so on and so on.
Scientists still do not know precisely what message is contained in each urine mark, but it seems highly likely that dogs’ urine carries an odor that is unique to each individual—one that can be memorized by others. It is also likely that in male dogs this unique odor contains contributions from the preputial gland as well as from the urine itself. What is less clear is how much other information is conveyed. For example, can a dog tell how large, how old, how hungry, how anxious, how confident another dog is, simply by sniffing its scent-mark? We cannot yet answer this question, but we do know that the main message carried by a bitch’s urine, apart from her identity, comes from the vaginally produced scents that indicate the status of her reproductive cycle. Bitches who are willing to mate produce a powerful pheromone that can attract males from long distances. (Pheromones are chemical signals that are similar in all individuals of a species.) However, scent does have one serious flaw as a communication medium—namely, that the message itself is very hard to control. Mammalian scent-signals are mainly produced by specialized skin glands. These glands inevitably get invaded by microorganisms, which alter the scent by adding metabolic products of their own, which can be pungent. If you had a nose as sensitive as a dog’s, it would be like putting up a notice in front of your house where graffiti artists come along at unpredictable times and progressively alter whatever is on the sign, including obliterating your own name.
Some animals, dogs included, have handed over responsibility for producing the smell to the microorganisms themselves. For example, bacteria on the mother’s skin make the odor that newborn puppies use to orientate toward their mothers. Likewise, the anal glands of both male and female dogs (and many other carnivores) secrete a mixture of fats and proteins into the anal sacs to which they are attached, allowing the bugs to turn these into the more volatile chemicals that make up the odor itself. (Scientists have shown that if antibiotics are injected into the sacs, killing the microorganisms, the secretion becomes almost odorless.) The “graffiti artists” can now write what they like, but they can use only the “colors” (fats and proteins) they’re given; thus the dog retains an element of control.
Such an odor is, however, both arbitrary and ever-fluctuating, placing limits on its usefulness. It is impossible to predict what it will smell like in advance, so if it is to be of any use in transmitting information, recipients will first have to learn what it means, and then relearn whenever it changes. Scent-marks that are intended to claim ownership of territory present an additional problem, since the whole point of them is to permanently identify individuals who are absent. If the odor of an owner’s scent-mark changes from one week to the next, an intruder may mistakenly deduce that ownership of the territory has recently changed when in fact it has not.
A territory-holder can overcome this drawback by occasionally actually meeting his neighbors, thereby giving them the opportunity to make the connection between his appearance and his scent. If that scent is changing subtly over time, then those meetings need to be frequent enough for the connection to be maintained. This behavior is known as “scent-matching”; widespread in rodents and in antelope, it is less widely studied in carnivores, and not at all in dogs, despite there being every indication that they must be doing something like it.
Making and reinforcing the link between odor and appearance seem to be uppermost in many dogs’ minds whenever they meet, suggesting that dogs do engage in a type of scent-matching. Specifically,
it’s likely that they memorize the odors of all the dogs they meet (Why else go to the trouble of all that sniffing?) and then compare these with all the indirect information that they get from sniffing scent-marks while they’re out on walks. If they don’t find any match, then they may assume that the other dog lives far away; if they find a lot of matches, then the dog must live nearby. Since scent-matching is usually connected with territorial behavior, perhaps domestic dogs perceive public parks and streets as a vast “no-man’s-land” between territories, always worth checking for occupancy in case they ever get the chance to live there.
Left to their own devices, many dogs prefer to use their sense of smell even when vision would appear to be more efficient. Trained explosives search dogs always prefer to use their noses rather than their eyes, even in cases where visual cues might lead them to their target more quickly. However, dogs are also very flexible in their behavior—and pet dogs quickly come to realize that we humans are much more attuned to visual cues than to olfactory ones. As a result, dogs can be successfully fooled into choosing an empty bowl rather than a bowl full of food—simply by having the dogs’ owner point to the empty one.12 Normally, of course, the dogs could have quickly identified the full bowl from its smell. This exemplifies the high priority that dogs put on social information, and also how well-adapted they are to attending to the ways we, as well as they, communicate.
Moreover, since dogs can tell each other apart by smell, they can surely learn the characteristic odors of the humans they live with or meet on a regular basis. They can probably also tell a great deal about our moods from the way these odor cues vary. Dogs can be specially trained to alert epileptic or unstable diabetic owners when they are about to have a seizure or hypoglycemic attack. There is little doubt that they do this by reacting to changes in the owner’s odor (although minute changes in “body-language,” undetectable to human observers, may also be part of the cue). Even ordinary pet dogs can be trained to serve this purpose; no special olfactory ability seems to be necessary.13 The implication is that all dogs are at least potentially able to monitor our moods based on the ways our body odor changes. (Of course, they must simultaneously allow for, and possibly try to interpret, other causes of our changing odor, such as our state of health and the different foods that we have eaten.) If so, they are picking up, and presumably reacting to, a vast range of information about our lives that we ourselves are only dimly aware of.
Perhaps one reason we can be so oblivious to the importance our dogs place on smell is how little they seem to suffer as a result of our ignorance. However, just as their ears can be damaged by the high levels of ultrasound produced by the clanging of metal kennel gates and furniture, dogs’ noses must surely be insulted by what must seem to them to be the overpowering odors of our detergents, fabric softeners, and “room fragrances.” Presumably they just get used to them, accepting them as an unavoidable downside of sharing a living space with the humans they are so closely bonded to. In the hygiene-conscious world we live in, many of us don’t like to let dogs do what they must feel compelled to do when they first meet us, which is to sniff us. I always hold out my hand to any dog I’m introduced to (I make a loose fist first, just in case the dog has a habit of nipping fingers). If the dog wants to lick my hand as well as sniff it, then I let him—I can always wash my hand later if I want to. Not doing so would be as unsociable as hiding our face from someone we’re being introduced to.
Perhaps it’s just as well that we have only recently started to become aware of this “secret world” that dogs inhabit; otherwise, we might be tempted to interfere with it. We have certainly taken liberties with their visual communication, by breeding them into such diverse shapes and sizes. The potential for a Chihuahua and a Great Dane to misunderstand each other’s visual signals seems almost unlimited, since they do not look like one another; nor, indeed, does either look much like a wolf. However, their scent glands, and the behavior that enables them to use these to communicate effectively, seem for all intents and purposes to be intact. It’s quite possible that dogs’ reliance on scent has been their salvation, enabling even breeds that look extraordinarily different from one another to go on conversing with one another—at some rather basic, smelly level.
CHAPTER 10
Problems with Pedigrees
Throughout most of this book, I’ve discussed dogs as if they were all roughly equivalent. And for our purposes, this is very often true: Despite some inevitable variations between breeds, all dogs share an evolutionary past, an acute sense of smell, a capacity for forming strong bonds with people, and an ability to recognize one another as members of the same species and to interact with one another accordingly. However, dogs are self-evidently not all the same, and sometimes it’s the differences between them that affect their well-being the most. The differences between dogs are primarily imposed by us, not by the dogs themselves. Where humankind doesn’t interfere with breeding, dogs look pretty much the same; village dogs in Africa, for instance, are essentially indistinguishable. They evolve into a type that is adapted to the environment they find themselves in. When humans start to choose which dogs to breed from, however, they generate dogs who are, by definition, less well suited to that niche. Initially, this probably didn’t matter at all. The capabilities that equip dogs to live on the street, alongside mankind, have gradually been replaced by those that allow dogs to live with man. These include not only changes that enable dogs to earn their living, such as helping with herding, hunting, and guarding (to name but three examples) but also those that enable dogs to be good companions.
However, as this process continued, there must have been many dogs whose well-being was compromised by humankind’s attempts to produce more extreme forms. For example, the Romans bred their mastiffs larger and larger, striving for fiercer and fiercer dogs. Some of these dogs must have been freaks—puppies too large to pass through their mother’s pelvis, or dogs whose skeletons were too heavy for their joints and thus in constant pain. In those rough-and-ready days, with precious little veterinary care, a crude kind of natural selection would have prevailed. Dogs who were not viable would have been stillborn or would not have lived long enough to breed, and dogs too infirm to perform their intended task would not have been selected for breeding.
Changes in rates of development have led to today’s extremes of size and shape
Like all animals, dogs are capable of producing far more offspring than is necessary for the continuation of their species. Unless a population is growing rapidly, this inevitably means that many individuals die before reproducing. As a general rule, the ones who die first are those least well suited to their environment. Many will have suffered before they died. This of course applies as much to village dogs as it does to dogs being bred by man for specific purposes. Nevertheless, the generation of new forms of dog inevitably left casualties in its wake.
Times have changed. In the West, we now believe in the right of each individual dog not to suffer. Puppies are no longer regarded as disposable items, to be drowned if unwanted. There is an outcry in the media whenever any cull of dogs is proposed, whether these be ferals, strays, or unwanted pets.
These are high standards indeed. We have taken upon ourselves the obligation to ensure that every puppy is wanted and will grow to be a healthy, happy dog. In many ways, we have succeeded in fulfilling these responsibilities. We have developed sufficient veterinary care to enable the majority of dogs to lead healthy lives. High-quality nutrition specifically designed for dogs is available in every supermarket, to the point that they have a healthier diet than some people do.
In other ways, however, we have failed our canine companions. In our seemingly insatiable quest for novelty, we have bred dogs who suffer from a vast range of avoidable ailments. And in our anthropomorphic need to see dogs as extensions of our own personalities, we have generated dogs that are unacceptably aggressive or have other temperamental defects. Their role as companions—a role they must fill if
they are to be assured of leading physically and psychologically healthy lives—seems rarely to be the first priority. Novice owners can be faced with choosing between pedigree puppies, primarily bred for appearance rather than temperament, and rescue dogs of uncertain parentage, many of whom will have been abandoned because they are the progeny of dogs bred for aggression.
Whereas dogs once made their own decisions about reproduction, nowadays in the West most matings are planned by humans. Over the past hundred years, specialists have increasingly come to control dog breeding. At present, most of our pet dogs either are pedigree dogs or can trace their ancestry just a few generations back to crosses between pedigree animals. By comparison with the whole history of the domestic dog, this is a very recent phenomenon, and one that is geographically and culturally confined: Genuinely ancient types of dog persist in many parts of the world. Nevertheless, most of the dogs available as pets to Westerners have pedigree ancestors.
The current rules for breeding pedigree dogs are causing profound and accelerating harm to their genetic viability. The registration systems for pedigree dogs in the United Kingdom, the United States, and many other parts of the world confine each breed to mate only with other members of the same breed. If this system were to be imposed totally, each breed would become completely genetically isolated from all other dogs. (In actuality, owing to unplanned matings and occasional deliberate crossbreedings, only the pedigree breeds themselves have been sequestered in this way.)
Although most of these breeding regulations are very new—affecting only the most recent one percent of the whole evolutionary history of the dog—they are already having profound effects on the dogs we see today. The genetic isolation of each breed has brought about a dramatic change in the dog’s gene pool, massively reducing the amount of variation in each breed. The less the variation, the more likely it is that damaging mutations will affect the welfare of individual dogs: In order for a potentially detrimental mutation to actually cause harm, it generally has to have been present in both parents—and this is likely to occur only if the parents themselves are closely related. In some dog breeds today it is difficult to find two parents who are not closely related.