Book Read Free

The Seven Deadly Virtues

Page 3

by Jonathan V. Last


  If you judge by the number of apocalyptic movies released lately, there’s no hope in popular culture either. Somebody’s probably working on an end-of-the-world remake of The Sound of Music.

  DOE, endangered species deer

  RAY, the earth collides with sun

  ME, the only person left

  FAR, Zombies! I’d better run

  SO, what am I going to do?

  LA, the town this crap comes from

  TEE, been there, got the shirt too

  Which brings teens back to see …

  Movies like this over and over again.

  7. Charity, however, we do not lack. As long as it’s tax deductible.

  Or go to any wedding. No matter how godless is the couple, the groomsmen, the bridesmaids, or for that matter the officiant, you’ll be forced to endure a bowdlerized version of 1 Corinthians 13 that goes something like this:

  Though I speak with the tongues of men and angels, and have not Amnesty International, I am become as sounding brass. PETA suffereth long, and is kind; Doctors Without Borders envieth not; the Sierra Club vaunteth not itself, is not puffed up. Planned Parenthood is not easily provoked, thinketh no evil; UNICEF beareth all things, believeth all things, endureth all things. And now abideth Nature Conservancy, Make-A-Wish Foundation, Habitat for Humanity, these three; but the greatest of these is United Way.

  Of course, usually the Revised Standard Version of the Bible is used, with the word “love” in place of “charity.” They’re not the same thing. In Greek (which was the language of Saint Paul in his letters to the Greeks in Corinth) there are four words for love. Eros is love in almost the only way we talk about “love” anymore except (let us invoke the virtue of hope) when we say we love our children. In that case, we mean storge, or affection. “I love you, dude,” is philia, or friendship. The word Saint Paul uses, however, is agape, the unconditional love of God and all his creation. Invoking the virtues of hope and faith, let’s suppose that’s what they’re doing at the United Way.

  Which points to the nonprofit nature of practicing the virtues in the modern world. Virtue survives. It just doesn’t provide modern Americans with the minimum compensation that they feel is necessary to meet their basic needs.

  Prudence keeps you out of the stock market. Justice costs like heck in legal bills. Fortitude is expensive, what with the cost of mixed drinks these days. Temperance, ditto, what with the cost of Promises in Malibu. Faith is broke—broken when the Democrats caved in on a budget deal that didn’t extend unemployment benefits. He who dines on hope is sure to lose weight. And charity begins at home. This is why you’re still living in your mother’s basement.

  The wages of sin may be death, but the wages of virtue are $7.25 per hour. Unless Congress changes the law.

  CHAPTER 2

  Prudence

  Long Live the Queen

  Andrew Ferguson

  PRUDENCE IS an intellectual virtue, the moral theologians tell us, sometimes even called the “queen of the virtues.” Without its power to humble and restrain, many other virtues collapse. This shouldn’t be news to us. We live in a time when prudence often goes missing, taking lots of things with it.

  The lack of prudence breeds a kind of silliness that only very sophisticated people are capable of; certain forms of ignorance are available exclusively to intellectuals who know so much that they have forgotten how much they don’t know. Into this category—which is really very crowded, and getting more crowded by the day—I place an evolutionary psychologist named Satoshi Kanazawa.

  Kanazawa’s field, evolutionary psychology, is part of what has been tagged the New Science, the application of the methodologies of the physical sciences to realms of human behavior—our motives, habits, morality, emotions, instincts, likes, and dislikes—that had until recently been considered beyond the reach of clinical experiment and computation. Once upon a time, we understood our social and interior lives through philosophy, religion, art, poetry, music, mythology, storytelling—all the wellsprings of humanism. Now we forage in such fields as “sociobiology,” “behavioral economics,” “social psychology,” “neuroeconomics,” “cognitive sociology,” even “experimental philosophy,” to plumb the deepest truths about ourselves.

  Kanazawa is a popularizer and booster of this New Science, as well as a practitioner. A professor at the London School of Economics, widely published in professional journals, he has for several years written a column, “The Scientific Fundamentalist,” for Psychology Today. The column carried a motto of his own choosing: “A Look at the Hard Truths about Human Nature.” Note these are hard truths he deals in. Like so many New Scientists, Kanazawa considers himself a wake-up-and-smell-the-coffee guy: a man bringing news of unavoidable, sometimes unpleasant, facts. He’s proud to say he’s willing to look reality in the eye. Unlike some people. You know who you are.

  In early 2011 Kanazawa devoted a column to one hard truth that nobody had dared to point out before—indeed, a hard truth that nobody had even suspected was true. Kanazawa alone had been brave enough to make the necessary scientific inquiry.

  “Women on average are more physically attractive than men,” he announced. This, it should be said, is kind of true, though it’s truer to say that in many surveys women as well as men tend to rate pictures of women more highly than pictures of men.

  “So women of all races,” Kanazawa went on, “are more physically attractive than the ‘average’ … except for black women.” The italics are his. In case readers failed to catch on, the editors of Psychology Today provided a helpfully question-begging headline: “Why are African American women less physically attractive than other women?”

  Being a good evolutionary psychologist, Kanazawa festooned his column with multicolored graphs and packed it with elaborate descriptions of his methodology. He had reached his remarkable conclusion, he revealed, by using a dataset from Add Health, a government-sponsored survey that interviewed adolescents over a period of years as they passed into adulthood. Among other things, interviewers rated the kids on their physical attractiveness.

  “I can compute the latent ‘physical attractiveness factor’ by using a statistical procedure called factor analysis,” he wrote. “The latent physical attractiveness factor has a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1.” Latent factors, statistical procedures, standard deviations … what could be more authoritative? The scientist allowed himself discursive asides: “It is very interesting to note that, even though black women are objectively less attractive than other women, black women and men subjectively consider themselves to be more physically attractive.” Very interesting indeed, Herr Doctor! And of course he proposed an evolutionary theory to account for the hard truth to which his factor analysis led him. It sounded terribly scientific, it really did: “The race differences in the level of testosterone can potentially explain why black women are less physically attractive than women of other races.” This time the italics are mine.

  Now, a few characteristics immediately leap out at any reader unlucky enough to happen upon Kanazawa’s piece. First among them is this: It is the work of a man unconstrained by prudence of any kind—intellectual, moral, social. There is the grandiosity, his unexamined confidence that the truths of human behavior can be captured in numbers and under controlled conditions. And there is the obvious methodological flaw: He uses subjective rankings by anonymous questioners of unidentified research subjects as a proxy for large, objective facts about entire classes of people. There is his pose as a disinterested observer, shrugging on his lab coat and pretending to follow the data wherever it leads him. And above all there is his reductionism, as though the conclusion he’s drawn about a made-up “physical attractiveness factor” could scarcely be otherwise thanks to the chemical composition of the human body, as it has been brought to us by natural selection.

  In one sense, of course, Kanazawa is an extreme case. Psychology Today’s editors dropped him as a columnist after more than sixty of his fellow evolutio
nary psychologists published an outraged letter insisting that Kanazawa “does not represent evolutionary psychology.” And this was also true, in a small way. Evolutionary psychology is one of the most fashionable of the disciplines in the New Science, and as a rule it attracts only the most careful careerists. None of them would be capable of the indiscretion that goosed Kanazawa into publishing a “finding” that (he must have known) most readers would find not only ludicrous but offensive.

  But that demonstrates a lack of prudence in the most superficial sense. On a deeper level, Kanazawa represents the New Science very well; he is in fact an exemplar. The reductionism, the dubious methodology, the touching faith in numbers: They all indicate a profound lack of intellectual modesty. Boorishness, the very opposite of prudence, is the hallmark of the New Science.

  We should be careful to distinguish the New Science from the physical sciences, which it resembles only in pantomime. The astonishing success of the physical sciences, from molecular biology to astrophysics, is what gives the New Scientists the confidence to pretend they’re doing the same thing the big boys are doing. The confidence, as we’ll see, is badly misplaced. Experiments that furnish the data for the New Science lack the test tubes, the microscopes, the particle accelerators that so impress us laymen about traditional science. Because the New Science takes as its subject such hard-to-pin-down phenomena as thoughts, motives, mental impressions, emotional reactions, and so on, its data are rather more elusive, too.

  Most often the data are generated straight from the classrooms and psych labs of major universities. Commonly, a varying number of undergraduates are asked by graduate students or their professors to pretend they are in one artificial situation or another, take tests about their reaction to the made-up situations, and then collect a modest sum of money or course credit for their trouble. At an apparently higher level of rigor—but not really—the students do the same thing while magnetic resonance images are made, resulting in colorful pictures of brains lighting up like Clark Griswold’s Christmas tree. The data yielded by these or other implausible methods are tortured by statistical analysis into the equations and charts and graphs that crowd the impenetrable prose of the published paper.

  And then, at last, comes the insight into how and why humans do what they do. Let’s consider some findings of the New Science—all of them breathlessly reported by a gullible media with lots of airtime and massive editorial holes to fill. No phrase in modern journalism is so overused as “studies show,” unless it’s “research reveals” or “experts say.”

  As a general rule, the findings of the New Science fall into one of two categories: They are trivial when true, and untrue when not trivial. One would have thought that Aristotle, for instance, had pretty much nailed the subject of happiness. He spent a whole book in his Ethics thinking about it! But now the old Greek egghead has been outdone. In Berkeley, California, the university’s Greater Good Science Center employs researchers in, yes, happiness studies. Specialists have engaged undergraduates in countless role-playing experiments. (At Berkeley, samples of undergraduates are disproportionately white or Asian, female, and unusually brainy, but they are taken as representative of humanity in general.)

  Through role-playing, the happy scientists have discovered that delaying gratification may lead to higher levels of oxytocin than simply satisfying desires as they arise. Oxytocin is a little reward cooked up through natural selection to make us happy. The ever-ready MRI machines have discovered that performing acts of kindness “lights up the left prefrontal cortex.” Other surveys of undergraduates tell us that a group performing various tasks skillfully provokes more favorable chemical reactions in their frontal lobes than a group that performs the same tasks poorly.

  So now we know, thanks to the New Science, that if you want to be happy, you should delay gratification, be kind to others, and manage your affairs competently. It’s science. Who knew?

  Well, down the ages, lots of people knew this—and they discovered it without access to psych majors or MRIs. But to the ear tuned to twenty-first-century buzz-buzz, the truisms of common sense and tradition sound much more authoritative if they’re preceded by the magic words “Studies show…”

  A pile of studies in the New Science has been rising on my desk over the last few months. All of them are certified by scientists at top universities. Here we go:

  An online survey of 334 subjects shows that parents accidentally confuse children’s names more often when the names of the children sound alike. MRIs from Western University in Ontario show that students well-trained in arithmetic are “better equipped to score higher on PSAT math.” Economic researchers “applied a Two Stage Least Squares methodology” to a set of real estate listings and discovered that “unattractive real estate agents achieve quicker sales.” A pair of evolutionary psychologists at Bristol University studied the “beer goggle effect”—the reputed tendency of men to find less attractive women more appealing after a few drinks—and discovered that it had evolutionary roots. (In the New Science everything must have evolutionary roots.) It seems that our earliest ancestors learned to value symmetry in facial composition because it is a signal of a healthy reproductive capacity; in time we humans took to calling this “beautiful.” Alcohol blurs our genetically controlled ability to judge symmetry, hence the beer goggle effect. Katy, bar the door!

  It’s tough to choose a favorite from my stack, but I’ll do it anyway: It relates to the evolutionary psychology dogma that men prefer a woman who has a waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) of 0.7. According to standard New Science method, this preference has been taken to be a universal impulse bred into our prehistoric ancestors and survives today as an evolutionary adaptation. Now, as it happens, the preference is not universal; men in China seem to favor a WHR of 0.8, for instance. Such an inconvenient fact does not deter the march of evolutionary psychology, however, because its practitioners have already come up with a theory to account for the universally preferred ratio.

  So what’s behind the WHR? Women with a 0.7 ratio very often have higher levels of estrogen, indicating fertility and general good health. Evolution thus favored men who favored these women, and a universal fact of human nature, a conception of beauty, was bred into our genes. This is an excellent example of New Science reasoning. Strained logic arises from a shaky factual basis to reach a conclusion that is simply an extension of the premise: Evolutionary psychology assumes that all basic human behavioral traits are adaptive, and therefore the WHR preference must have been adaptive.

  But there’s more! To confirm the theory about WHR, two evolutionary psychologists at Maryhurst College came up with another experiment: If the WHR theory is true, then stressful circumstances, which make men less inclined to reproduce, should alter their preference to a less estrogen-friendly ratio. On went the lab coats, out came the spreadsheets, and the two researchers gathered up every cover of Playboy magazine from 1960 to 2000. Yes, they really did. And they discovered that in periods of economic recession, the WHR of the bunnies on the cover rose from the standard 0.7. In times of stress, men liked heavier women, according to the Bunnies. QED, as the Romans used to say; “research reveals,” as we say.

  Such reasoning brings us the “fat gene,” “the morality molecule,” “the infidelity neuron,” and all of the other discoveries that parade across the pages of Time and Psychology Today and the stage sets of Today and Good Morning America. A neurosurgeon at UCLA has even discovered the “Jennifer Aniston neuron,” which controls a man’s reaction to the aging starlet. Jealous colleagues are now in pursuit of the Julia Roberts neuron and the Halle Berry neuron. You think I’m kidding? The New Science would indeed be merely comical—the credulity of smart people is endlessly amusing—but for two considerations.

  First, our infatuation with a reductionist, materialist conception of life has real-world effects, in marketing, business organization, even public policy. Some of the effects are more serious than others. A few years ago, interior decorators across Europe
and America began painting corporate headquarters and work spaces green after researchers “discovered” that green was the color most conducive to creativity. (The evolutionary explanation: On the prehistoric savannah, our ancestors learned to associate green with water, nutrition, plant life, and, of course, fertility.) The painters had to be called in again after a year or two, when other researchers (working on a different group of undergraduates, no doubt) found that exposure to the color blue “can double your creative output.” (The new evolutionary explanation: Our cleverest ancestors were stimulated by the cerulean sky, the azure sea….)

  But more ominously, the New Science is invading government, too. “Behavioral economics,” whose practitioners swallow the findings of social psychology without a second thought, is now the guiding principle of much economic policymaking. There is no reason why this should be so. Behavioral economists were as clueless as old-fashioned economists, for example, in failing to foresee the financial collapse of 2008, the single most calamitous economic event of the past eighty years. Nevertheless, President Obama called in the behaviorists when it came time to design his “middle-class tax cut” in 2009. It was supposed to goose consumer spending, and hence economic growth.

  The tax cut was an exercise in “framing.” New Scientists are mad for “framing.” They believe (studies show) that how an object, idea, or event is presented, described, or packaged determines how people will react to it. With the tax cut the question was how the money should be placed in taxpayers’ hands. A lump-sum “tax rebate” in 2001 had failed to stimulate the economy (research revealed). But if the tax cut were made slowly—by changing the rate of withholding in workers’ pay-checks—then they would be more likely to spend it.

 

‹ Prev