The Ravenous Brain: How the New Science of Consciousness Explains Our Insatiable Search for Meaning

Home > Other > The Ravenous Brain: How the New Science of Consciousness Explains Our Insatiable Search for Meaning > Page 37
The Ravenous Brain: How the New Science of Consciousness Explains Our Insatiable Search for Meaning Page 37

by Bor, Daniel


  P. Haggard, Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat Rev Neurosci , 2008. 9(12): 934–946.

  W. P. Banks and S. Pockett, Benjamin Libet’s work on the neuroscience of free will, in The Blackwell companion to consciousness, M. Velmans and S. Schneider, eds. 2007, Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 657–670.

  104 Consciousness was smeared across time

  D. C. Dennett, Consciousness explained. 1991, New York: Penguin.

  104 Computational model by Stanislav Nikolov

  S. Nikolov, D. A. Rahnev, and H. C. Lau, Probabilistic model of onset detection explains previous puzzling findings in human time perception. Front Psychol, 2010. 1: 37.

  105 Detected up to 10 seconds prior to the conscious decision

  C. S. Soon et al., Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nat Neurosci, 2008. 11(5): 543–545.

  CHAPTER 4: PAY ATTENTION TO THAT PATTERN!

  111 “. . . Mummy sent me to fetch you”

  I. Stewart, Professor Stewart’s hoard of mathematical treasures. 2009, London: Profile Books.

  113 Brain . . . half of all the energy the child consumes

  M. A. Holliday, Body composition and energy needs during growth, in Human growth: a comprehensive treatise, F. Falkner and J. M. Tanner, eds. 1986, New York: Plenum, 101–117.

  113 Human brain . . . nearing the endpoint . . . biologically possible

  D. Fox, The limits of intelligence. Sci Am, 2011. 305(1): 36–43.

  114 Two photos identical except for one feature

  R. A. Rensink, J. K. O’Regan, and J. J. Clark, To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol Sci, 1997. 8(5): 368–373.

  115 Half the volunteers notice . . . person has changes

  D. J. Simons and D. T. Levin, Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychon Bull Rev, 1998. 5(4): 644–649.

  116 44 percent of people actually notice the gorilla

  D. J. Simons and C. F. Chabris, Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 1999. 28(9): 1059–1074.

  117 Conscious of fainter targets . . . also detect targets faster

  R. Desimone and J. Duncan, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci, 1995. 18: 193–222.

  J. W. Bisley and M. E. Goldberg, Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science, 2003. 299(5603): 81–86.

  M. Carrasco, C. Penpeci-Talgar, and M. Eckstein, Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement. Vision Res, 2000. 40(10–12): 1203–1215.

  117 Object will be perceived . . . more contrast

  M. Carrasco, S. Ling, and S. Read, Attention alters appearance. Nat Neurosci , 2004. 7(3): 308–313.

  118 Attention emerging from . . . collective neuronal war

  Desimone and Duncan, Neural mechanisms.

  J. Duncan, EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention. Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester), 2006. 59(1): 2–27.

  119 Famous student physics lectures

  R. P. Feynman, R. B. Leighton, and M. Sands, Lectures on physics: complete set, vols. 1–3. 1998, Boston: Addison Wesley.

  122 Ferret brains . . . rewired . . . visual cortex in blind . . . process Braille

  J. Sharma, A. Angelucci, and M. Sur, Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex. Nature, 2000. 404(6780): 841–847.

  L. Von Melchner, S. L. Pallas, and M. Sur, Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections directed to the auditory pathway. Nature, 2000. 404(6780): 871–876.

  H. Burton et al., Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading. J Neurophysiol, 2002. 87(1): 589–607.

  124 Constant competition . . . in the brain

  Desimone and Duncan (1995), see above.

  Duncan (2006), see above.

  E. I. Knudsen, Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci, 2007. 30: 57–78.

  127 Modern version of Phineas Gage

  A. R. Damasio, Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. 1994, New York: HarperCollins.

  129 Multiple personality disorder . . . attribute . . . experiences to other personalities

  E. Bliss, Multiple personality, allied disorders, and hypnosis. 1986, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  131 Aware of your own consciousness

  H. Lau and D. Rosenthal, Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends Cogn Sci, 2011. 15(8): 365–373.

  132 Poor at matching their confidence to their accuracy

  S. M. Fleming et al., Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science, 2010. 329(5998): 1541–1543.

  135 Three or four conscious items

  N. Cowan, The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci, 2001. 24(1): 87–114; discussion 114–185.

  135 “Global workspace theory” proposed by Bernard Baars

  B. J. Baars, In the theater of consciousness: the workspace of the mind. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press.

  B. J. Baars and S. Franklin, An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA. Neural Netw, 2007. 20(9): 955–961.

  136 George Sperling presented subjects

  G. Sperling, The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 1960. 74(11, Whole No. 498): 1–29.

  137 Steven Yantis presented subjects with

  S. Yantis, Multielement visual tracking: attention and perceptual organization. Cogn Psychol, 1992. 24(3): 295–340.

  137 Our working memory limit . . . same as the monkey’s

  E. Heyselaar, K. Johnston, and M. Paré, The capacity limit of the visual working memory of the macaque monkey. J Vision. 10(7): 725.

  138 Other species . . . same upper bound . . . honeybee

  H. J. Gross et al., Number-based visual generalisation in the honeybee. PLoS One, 2009. 4(1): e4263.

  138 3 or 4 items . . . maximum that can be practically sustained

  A. Raffone and G. Wolters, A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory. J Cogn Neurosci, 2001. 13(6): 766–785.

  139 Each holder . . . cope equally well . . . simplest . . . most complex

  J. Duncan, Similarity between concurrent visual discriminations: dimensions and objects. Percept Psychophys, 1993. 54(4): 425–430.

  140 Say back a novel sequence that was 80 digits

  K. A. Ericcson, W. G. Chase, and S. Falloon, Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 1980. 208: 1181–1182.

  142 Presented volunteers . . . sequences of 4 double digits

  D. Bor and A. M. Owen, A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory. Cereb Cortex, 2007. 17(4): 778–786.

  142 Chess masters . . . remember . . . whole board

  W. G. Chase and H. A. Simon, Perception in chess. Cogn Psychol, 1973. 4: 55–81.

  143 Increase the amount of information per item

  G. A. Miller, The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev, 1956. 63(2): 81–97.

  144 Greater the number, the less likely . . . identify each

  C. Bundesen, H. Shibuya, and A. Larsen, Visual selection from multielement displays: a model for partial report, in Attention and performance XI, M. I. Posner and M.S.I. Marin, eds. 1985, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 631–649.

  J. J. Todd and R. Marois, Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 2004. 428(6984): 751–754.

  144 Attention has two clear stages

  C. Bundesen, T. Habekost, and S. Kyllingsbaek, A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. Psychol Rev, 2005. 112(2): 291–328.

  145 Leonardo Chelazzi and colleagues used electrodes

  L. Chelazzi et al., Responses
of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search. J Neurophysiol, 1998. 80(6): 2918–2940.

  145 Michael Cohen and colleagues recently carried out

  M. A. Cohen, G. A. Alvarez, and K. Nakayama, Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychol Sci, 2011. 22(9): 1165–1172.

  146 Divert attention away . . . fails to enter consciousness

  J. S. Joseph, M. M. Chun, and K. Nakayama, Attentional requirements in a “preattentive” feature search task. Nature, 1997. 387(6635): 805–807.

  J. Theeuwes, A. F. Kramer, and P. Atchley, Attentional effects on preattentive vision: spatial precues affect the detection of simple features. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 1999. 25(2): 341–347.

  S. Walker, P. Stafford, and G. Davis, Ultra-rapid categorization requires visual attention: scenes with multiple foreground objects. J Vision, 2008. 8(4): 211–212.

  146 Working memory . . . is limited to . . . 4 . . . items

  Cowan (2001), see above.

  147 Psychologists assumed that our working memory capacity was around double this

  Miller (1956), see above.

  148 Douglas Hofstadter’s whimsical and influential book

  D. Hofstadter, Godel, Escher, Bach: an eternal golden braid (20th anniversary edition, with a new preface by the author). 1999, London: Penguin.

  150 Prior expectations . . . guide our attention

  L. Melloni et al., Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness. J Neurosci, 2011. 31(4): 1386–1396.

  C. Summerfield and T. Egner, Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends Cogn Sci, 2009. 13(9): 403–409.

  151 Other species can start to learn . . . grammatical language

  S. Savage-Rumbaugh and R. Lewin, Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind. 1994, New York: John Wiley & Sons.

  151 A “language instinct”

  S. Pinker, The language instinct: the new science of language and mind. 2003, London: Penguin.

  152 Learning an artificial grammar . . . same brain areas . . . chunking task

  D. Bor et al., Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron, 2003. 37(2): 361–367.

  P. Fletcher et al., Learning-related neuronal responses in prefrontal cortex studied with functional neuroimaging. Cereb Cortex, 1999. 9(2): 168–178.

  152 FOXP2

  F. Vargha-Khadem et al., FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language . Nat Rev Neurosci, 2005. 6(2): 131–138.

  154 The evolutionary advantage of awareness

  D. Bor and A. K. Seth, Consciousness and the prefrontal parietal network: insights from attention, working memory and chunking. Front Psychol, 2012. 3: 63.

  155 Stan Beilock and colleagues tested . . . goft-putting

  S. L. Beilock et al., Haste does not always make waste: expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychon Bull Rev, 2004. 11(2): 373–379.

  S. L. Beilock et al., When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. J Exp Psychol Appl, 2002. 8(1): 6–16.

  155 Similar results . . . soccer, baseball . . . touch typing

  B. Castaneda and R. Gray, Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. J Sport Exerc Psychol, 2007. 29(1): 60–77.

  G. D. Logan and M. J. Crump, The left hand doesn’t know what the right hand is doing: the disruptive effects of attention to the hands in skilled typewriting. Psychol Sci, 2009. 20(10): 1296–1300.

  CHAPTER 5: THE BRAIN’S EXPERIENCE OF A ROSE

  160 Woman was born . . . no cerebellum

  D. Timmann et al., Cerebellar agenesis: clinical, neuropsychological and MR findings. Neurocase, 2003. 9(5): 402–413.

  160 Cerebellum . . . 80 percent of . . . brain’s neurons

  F. A. Azevedo et al., Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain. J Comp Neurol, 2009. 513(5): 532–541.

  161 Isn’t enough neural coherence . . . aware . . . when . . . awake

  N. D. Schiff, Recovery of consciousness after brain injury: a mesocircuit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci, 2010. 33(1): 1–9.

  163 Similar stories occur for blindtouch

  L. Weiskrantz, Consciousness lost and found: a neuropsychological exploration . 1997, Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  163 A visual experience . . . primary visual cortex is missing

  D. H. Ffytche and S. Zeki, The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision. Brain, 2011. 134 (Pt 1): 247–257.

  163 Degraded form of consciousness . . . matches their reduced ability

  M. Overgaard et al., Seeing without seeing? Degraded conscious vision in a blindsight patient. PLoS One, 2008. 3(8): e3028.

  164 Blindsight patient in the fMRI scanner

  A. Sahraie et al., Pattern of neuronal activity associated with conscious and unconscious processing of visual signals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 1997. 94(17): 9406–9411.

  165 Nikos Logothetis and his team, who carried out

  D. A. Leopold and N. K. Logothetis, Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys’ percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature, 1996. 379(6565): 549–553.

  N. K. Logothetis, Single units and conscious vision. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 1998. 353(1377): 1801–1818.

  165 Perception . . . persist . . . momentary gap of the eye blink

  T. J. Gawne and J. M. Martin, Activity of primate V1 cortical neurons during blinks. J Neurophysiol, 2000. 84(5): 2691–2694.

  G. Rees, G. Kreiman, and C. Koch, Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2002. 3(4): 261–270.

  166 Primary visual cortex is not quite as dumb

  M. A. Silver, D. Ress, and D. J. Heeger, Neural correlates of sustained spatial attention in human early visual cortex. J Neurophysiol, 2007. 97(1): 229–237.

  168 Two other regions also light up at least as brightly

  E. D. Lumer, K. J. Friston, and G. Rees, Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science, 1998. 280(5371): 1930–1934.

  G. Rees, Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 2007. 362(1481): 877–886.

  168 Record . . . experiences switch . . . candlestick or faces

  A. Kleinschmidt et al., Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures. Proc Biol Sci, 1998. 265(1413): 2427–2433.

  168 Posterior parietal . . . always activated . . . with the lateral prefrontal

  J. Duncan, EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention. Q J Exp Psychol (Colchester), 2006. 59(1): 2–27.

  169 Stanislas Dehaene . . . showed subjects a rapid sequence

  S. Dehaene et al., Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat Neurosci, 2001. 4(7): 752–758.

  169 Touch or a sound, or . . . combination of senses

  S. Dehaene and J. P. Changeux, Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 2011. 70(2): 200–227.

  169 Physical size . . . brain structure . . . links with awareness

  S. M. Fleming et al., Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science, 2010. 329(5998): 1541–1543.

  R. Kanai, B. Bahrami, and G. Rees, Human parietal cortex structure predicts individual differences in perceptual rivalry. Current Biol, 2010. 20(18): 1626–1630.

  169 Antoine Del Cul and colleagues gave patients

  A. Del Cul et al., Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain, 2009. 132 (Pt 9): 2531–2540.

  170 Jon Simons and colleagues gave a memory test

  J. S. Simons et al., Dissociation between memory accuracy and memory confidence following bilateral parietal lesions.
Cereb Cortex, 2010. 20(2): 479–485.

  170 Matt Davis and colleagues played volunteers . . . sentences

  M. H. Davis et al., Dissociating speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2007. 104(41): 16032–16037.

  170 The lowest share compared to our primate cousins

  A. A. De Sousa et al., Hominoid visual brain structure volumes and the position of the lunate sulcus. J Hum Evol, 2010. 58(4): 281–292.

 

‹ Prev