Book Read Free

The Lives of Bees

Page 14

by Thomas D Seeley


  comb that is held in 13 of the full- size (deep) frames that fit in a Langstroth

  hive. Most of each colony’s comb was, as expected, the smaller- celled

  worker comb, but there was also a sizable portion of the larger- cell drone

  comb, 17 percent, on average (range, 10%–24%). I regret to report that

  we did not measure the sizes of the worker cells in these bee- tree nests.

  But working from photographs of the combs in three of these wild colo-

  nies’ nests (e.g., Fig. 3.7), I have determined the mean wall- to- wall dimen-

  sion of worker cells in the brood combs of three of the wild colony’s nests:

  5.12, 5.19, and 5.25 millimeters (0.201, 0.204, and 0.206 inch). So, on

  average, their worker cell size was 5.19 millimeters. For comparison, in

  my managed colonies, which have built their combs on standard beeswax

  comb foundation purchased from various manufacturers, the worker cells

  have a somewhat larger mean wall- to- wall dimension: 5.38 millimeters

  (0.212 inch). Once I did a study that looked at small- cell comb as a means

  of controlling infestations of Varroa mites (discussed in chapter 10), and in

  the pilot- work stage of this study I induced colonies to build combs on

  small- cell foundation. The cells of the worker comb built by these colonies

  had an average wall- to- wall dimension of just 4.82 millimeters (0.190

  inch). We can conclude, therefore, that the wild- colony nests studied in

  the mid- 1970s contained worker comb whose cell size was between that

  of the standard- cell worker comb and small- cell worker comb found in

  beekeepers’ hives but much closer to the former than the latter.

  The bees in these wild colonies had organized the use of their combs in

  the way that is familiar to all beekeepers, storing honey in the upper region

  of the nest, rearing brood below, and creating a band of cells holding pol-

  Seeley.indb 108

  2/21/2019 8:07:33 AM

  The Nest 109

  len in between, though often with additional cells of pollen scattered

  within the colony’s brood- nest region. Most of the nests were collected

  and dissected in late summer—late July and August—and except for one

  colony that was queenless, all were thriving. The average sizes of the

  worker bee and drone populations of the queenright colonies were 17,800

  and 1,004, respectively. It was clear that the colonies’ production of more

  workers and drones would soon be limited by the filling of their combs

  with honey. On average, the colonies had 15.1 kilograms (33.2 pounds)

  of honey stores and had already filled roughly 50 percent of the cells in

  their nests with honey: 56 percent of the worker cells and 48 percent of

  the drone cells. They had brood in 25 percent of their worker- comb cells

  and 26 percent of their drone- comb cells, so 19 percent of their worker

  cells and 26 percent of their drone cells were vacant. Overall, it appeared

  that these colonies were making good progress toward having the 25- plus

  kilograms (55 pounds) of honey stores and the large population of young

  bees that each would need to survive the winter. We examined all the

  combs closely for signs of brood diseases—American foulbrood, Euro-

  pean foulbrood, sacbrood, and chalkbrood—but found none. Our study

  was conducted some 10 years before the first detections of tracheal mites

  ( Acarapis woodi, in Florida, in 1984) and Varroa mites ( Varroa destructor, in Florida, in 1987) in North America, so naturally we did not find either of

  these two parasites.

  NEST- SITE SELECTION

  The tree cavity or rock crevice that houses a wild colony’s nest is the cen-

  ter of the universe for its inhabitants. It is the spot where these bees have

  built their nest, the place they will defend with their lives, and the only

  site on earth to which they return from miles around bearing loads of

  nectar and pollen. Both the nesting site and the beeswax combs inside are

  parts of the colony’s set of survival tools that extend beyond the bodies of

  its members. It is obvious to anyone who has peered inside a wild colony’s

  nest and admired its combs (Fig. 5.5) that these labyrinthine structures are

  products of the bees living there. After all, the beeswax used to build each

  Seeley.indb 109

  2/21/2019 8:07:33 AM

  Fig. 5.5. Combs in the nest of a honey bee colony living in a hollow tree.

  Seeley.indb 110

  2/21/2019 8:07:34 AM

  The Nest 111

  comb is a secretion of the bees’ bodies, and the marvelous hexagonal- cell

  structure of each comb is a product of the bees’ behavior. What is less obvi-

  ous, though, is that the hollow tree or rock pile that shelters this intricate

  nest is also part of the colony’s extended tool kit for survival. As we shall

  see, although honey bees do not build their nesting sites, they do carefully

  choose them, so the cavity that a colony occupies is also a product of its

  members’ behavior.

  The honey bee’s process of choosing a dwelling place unfolds during

  colony reproduction (swarming), which occurs mainly in late spring and

  early summer (May–July) in the Ithaca area. The first step in this house-

  hunting process begins even before a swarm has left the parent nest. A few

  hundred of a colony’s oldest bees, its foragers, cease collecting food and

  turn instead to scouting for new living quarters. This requires a radical

  switch in behavior. These bees no longer visit brightly lit, sweet- scented

  sources of nectar and pollen; instead they investigate dark places—knot-

  holes, cracks in tree limbs, gaps among roots, and crevices in rocks—

  always seeking a snug cavity suitable for housing a honey bee colony.

  Upon discovering a potential homesite, a scout spends nearly an hour

  examining it closely. Her inspection consists of a few dozen trips inside

  the cavity, each one lasting about one minute, alternating with trips out-

  side. While outside, the scout scurries over the nest structure around the

  entrance opening and performs slow, hovering flights all around the nest

  site, apparently conducting a detailed visual inspection of the structure and

  surrounding objects. While inside, the bee scrambles over the interior

  surfaces, at first not venturing far inside the cavity, but with increasing

  experience pressing deeper and deeper into the remote corners of the

  hollow. When her examination is complete, the scout will have walked

  some 50 meters (about 150 feet) or more inside the cavity and so will have

  crossed all its inner surfaces. Experiments that I conducted with a cylindri-

  cal nest box whose walls could be rotated freely while its bright entrance

  opening remained stationary—so, in effect, the scout stepped onto a

  treadmill when she went inside the nest box—have shown that a scout bee

  judges a potential nest cavity’s volume by sensing the amount of walking

  Seeley.indb 111

  2/21/2019 8:07:34 AM

  112 Chapter 5

  required to circumnavigate it. Exactly how a scout bee judges a cavity’s

  roominess using the information gained from the walking (and occasional

  flying) movements that she makes inside the cavity remains a mystery.

  The long du
ration of the nest- site inspections made by scout bees sug-

  gested that they assess multiple properties of a site to judge its suitability.

  Moreover, the regularities in certain nest- site properties that Roger Morse

  and I had found—in entrance area, entrance height, cavity volume, and so

  forth—supported the idea that bees have strong preferences in their hous-

  ing. It was also possible, however, that the consistencies we found merely

  reflected what was generally available in tree cavities. At this point, I

  turned to surveying the scientific and beekeeping literature for informa-

  tion about the nest- site preferences of honey bees, but I found almost

  nothing, just one article in a French beekeeping magazine on how to build

  attractive bait hives for catching wild swarms. This situation surprised me,

  because I knew that beekeepers had worked for centuries to design the

  perfect hive, and I figured they might have looked to the natural living

  quarters of honey bee colonies for guidance, but evidently they had not.

  At the same time, finding this gap in our knowledge delighted me, for I

  realized then that my curiosity about the bees’ natural homes had drawn

  me to a region of uncharted territory in the biology of Apis mellifera.

  The method I developed for asking the bees about their nest- site prefer-

  ences was simple: set out nest boxes that differed in certain properties and

  see which ones were occupied preferentially by wild swarms. More spe-

  cifically, I set out nest boxes in groups of two, three, or four, with the

  boxes in each group identical except for one property, such as entrance

  area or cavity volume. The boxes within each group were spaced about 10

  meters (33 feet) apart on similar- size trees (or a pair of power- line poles)

  where they were matched in their visibility, wind exposure, and location

  (Fig. 5.6). Each group of boxes served to test one (potential) nest- site

  preference, and it did so by giving swarms a choice between one box

  whose properties all matched those of a typical nest site in nature (e.g.,

  average entrance size, average cavity volume, etc.) and another box (or

  boxes) identical to the first box except in one property (e.g., entrance

  Seeley.indb 112

  2/21/2019 8:07:34 AM

  The Nest 113

  Fig. 5.6. Two nest boxes mounted on power- line poles. The two boxes offer

  identical nesting sites (same cavity volume and shape, same entrance height and

  direction, etc.), except that the one on the right has a smaller entrance opening

  (12.5 square centimeters/2 square inches) than the one on the left (75 square

  centimeters/12 square inches).

  size), the value of which was atypical. In this way, wild swarms were tested

  for a preference in the one variable in which the boxes differed. For ex-

  ample, to test whether the distribution of entrance areas shown in Figure

  5.2 reflects a preference for small entrance openings, I set up pairs of cubi-

  cal nest boxes that were identical except that one box had an entrance area

  of 12.5 square centimeters (ca. 2 square inches), which was typical, and

  the other box had a large entrance area of 75 square centimeters (ca. 12

  square inches, the size found in a Langstroth hive), which was atypical.

  Planning this investigation was easy, but executing it was hard. Alto-

  gether, I built 252 nest boxes in the winter of 1975–1976, and I deployed

  them in small groups over the countryside around Ithaca in the summers

  of 1976 and 1977. Luckily, wild swarms were plentiful, so the experimen-

  Seeley.indb 113

  2/21/2019 8:07:35 AM

  114 Chapter 5

  Table 5.1. Nest- site properties for which honey bees do or do not show preferences,

  based on nest- box occupations by swarms. A > B, denotes A preferred to B; A = B denotes no preference between A and B.

  Property

  Preference

  Function(s)

  Entrance size

  12.5 cm2 > 75 cm2

  Colony defense and thermoregulation

  Entrance direction

  south > north facing

  Colony thermoregulation

  Entrance height

  5 m > 1 m

  Colony defense

  Entrance location

  bottom > top of cavity

  Colony thermoregulation

  Entrance shape

  None: circle = slit

  (Both shapes work well)

  Cavity volume

  10 < 40 > 100 liters

  Storage space for honey; colony

  thermoregulation

  Cavity shape

  None: cubical = tall

  (Both shapes work well)

  Cavity dryness

  None: wet = dry

  (Bees can waterproof a leaky cavity)

  Cavity draftiness

  None: drafty = tight

  (Bees can caulk cracks and holes)

  Combs in cavity

  with > without

  Economy of comb construction

  tal plan worked. My nest boxes attracted 124 swarms, enough to reveal

  many of the bees’ secrets about what they seek in a homesite.

  Table 5.1 summarizes the results of this study. We see that the bees in

  these wild swarms revealed preferences in four aspects of the entrance

  openings of their nest cavities. This was not surprising, given that this pas-

  sageway is the interface between the colony and the rest of the world. All

  of a colony’s food, water, resin, and fresh air comes in through this open-

  ing; all its waste and debris goes out of it; and all attacks by predators will

  focus on this point of greatest vulnerability. We also see that the wild

  swarms expressed preferences about just two features of the cavity itself:

  its volume and whether it was furnished with beeswax combs (from a

  previous colony).

  FUNCTIONS OF THE BEES’ HOUSING PREFERENCES

  Entrance Size

  I set up 14 test sites where swarms had a choice between two nest boxes

  that differed only in entrance size, and at six of them a wild swarm occu-

  pied one of the boxes, always the one with the smaller, 12.5- square- centimeter

  Seeley.indb 114

  2/21/2019 8:07:35 AM

  The Nest 115

  Fig. 5.7. The right half of a hive entrance (2 × 14 centimeters/0.8 × 5.5 inches)

  that was reduced in late summer when the bees built a propolis wall over most

  of the opening, leaving just the two small passageways shown. The one on the left

  is barely wide enough for three bees to crawl through simultaneously.

  (2- square- inch) entrance opening. This was unsurprising, because a small

  entrance helps a colony defend itself against animals that want to steal its

  honey. Most beekeepers around Ithaca, for example, know to reduce the

  entrance openings of their hives in the autumn, especially once the frosts

  have destroyed the flowers, and the yellow jacket wasps ( Vespula spp.), now

  starving, try desperately to get to the bees’ stores of honey. A small en-

  trance probably also helps a colony stay warm in winter by minimizing

  draftiness in its nest cavity. This may also explain why some colonies, in

  late autumn, will reduce their nest’s entrance by closing off most of the

  opening with a propolis wall pierced by just a few openings, each one just

  large enough to allow pass
age of a bee or two (Fig. 5.7).

  Entrance Direction

  Test sites where the paired nest boxes faced southeast, south, or southwest

  had markedly higher probabilities of occupation than those where the

  boxes faced northwest, north, or northeast. Evidently, the bees prefer

  their nest entrances to have a southerly exposure. A study conducted by

  Seeley.indb 115

  2/21/2019 8:07:35 AM

  116 Chapter 5

  Tibor I. Szabo in Alberta, Canada, found that a hive with a south- facing

  entrance, relative to one with a north- facing entrance, had a lower prob-

  ability of becoming plugged by ice and snow in winter, and that having the

  hive entrance open all winter improved nest ventilation and colony health.

  A south- facing entrance probably also helps the bees by providing a solar-

  heated site from which they can take off to conduct cleansing flights—to

  eliminate accumulated body wastes—on mild days in midwinter.

  Entrance Height

  I established eight test sites with paired nest boxes that were either high

  and low, and I caught six swarms at them, all in the high box of the pair.

  This indicated a clear preference for nest entrances high off the ground,

  consistent with the distribution of entrance heights for the nests of wild

  colonies located by bee hunting (Fig. 5.2). In chapter 10, I will describe a

  natural experiment in the Arnot Forest that shows one way that a wild

  colony benefits from having its nest entrance high up: it lowers the risk of

  detection by bears. It may also reduce the likelihood of nest damage in

  winter by woodland rodents such as deer mice ( Peromyscus maniculatus).

  Entrance Location

  This variable was tested by setting out 12 pairs of nest boxes. Both boxes

  in each pair provided a cavity that was 100 centimeters (ca. 40 inches) tall

  and 20 centimeters (ca. 8 inches) wide and deep. But one box had its en-

  trance opening at floor level, while the other had it flush with the ceiling.

  Ten of these 12 pairs of nest boxes attracted a swarm: eight moved into

  the box with a bottom entrance, and two into the box with a top entrance.

  In chapter 9, we will look at a study by Derek M. Mitchell, an engineer

  and physicist, that sheds light on the benefits to the bees of not having an

  opening near the top of their nest cavity.

  Cavity Volume

 

‹ Prev