Book Read Free

Not Born Yesterday

Page 43

by Hugo Mercier


  psy chol ogy (pp. 323–390). Boston: McGraw- Hil .

  Pfaff, S. (2001). “The limits of coercive surveil ance: Social and penal control in the

  German Demo cratic Republic.” Punishment and Society, 3(3), 381–407.

  Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.

  Planck, M. (1968). Scientific autobiography and other papers (F. Gaynor, Trans.). New

  York: Citadel Press.

  Platow, M. J., Foddy, M., Yamagishi, T., Lim, L., & Chow, A. (2012). “Two experimental

  tests of trust in in- group strangers: The moderating role of common knowledge

  of group membership.” Eu ro pean Journal of Social Psy chol ogy, 42(1), 30–35.

  Pomper, G. M., & Lederman, S. S. (1980). Elections in Amer ica: Control and influence

  in demo cratic politics. New York: Longman.

  Porter, S., & ten Brinke, L. (2008). “Reading between the lies: Identifying concealed

  and falsified emotions in universal facial expressions.” Psychological Science, 19(5),

  508–514.

  Pound, J., & Zeckhauser, R. (1990). “Clearly heard on the street: The effect of take-

  over rumors on stock prices.” Journal of Business, 63(3), 291–308.

  Power, E. A. (2017). “Social support networks and religiosity in rural South India.”

  Nature Human Behaviour, 1(3), 0057.

  Prasad, J. (1935). “The psy chol ogy of rumour: A study relating to the great Indian earth-

  quake of 1934.” British Journal of Psy chol ogy. General Section, 26(1), 1–15.

  Pratkanis, A. R., & Aronson, E. (1992). Age of propaganda: The everyday use and abuse

  of persuasion. New York: W. H. Freeman.

  Priniski, J., & Horne, Z. (2018). “Attitude change on Reddit’s change my view.” Pro-

  ceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Conference.

  Proulx, G., Fahy, R. F., & Walker, A. (2004). Analy sis of first- person accounts from survivors

  of the World Trade Center evacuation on September 11. Retrieved from https:// s3

  . amazonaws .com / academia .edu .documents / 36860616 / Analysis _ of _ First

  - Person _ Accounts .PDF ? AWSAccessKeyId = AKIAIWOWYYGZ2Y53UL3A &

  Expires = 1542920752&Signature = S5zsNHIA%2BObbcYJA%2BSBpXT%2BGrR

  8%3D&response - content - disposition = inline%3B%20filename%3DAnalysis _ of

  _ First - Person _ Accounts _ PDF .pdf

  Pulford, B. D., Colman, A. M., Buabang, E. K., & Krockow, E. M. (2018). “The per-

  suasive power of knowledge: Testing the confidence heuristic.” Journal of Experi-

  mental Psy chol ogy: General, 147(10), 1431–1444.

  r e f e r e n c e s 339

  Puschmann, C. (2018, November). “Beyond the bubble: Assessing the diversity of

  po liti cal search results.” Digital Journalism, doi: https:// doi .org / 10 .1080 / 21670811

  . 2018 .1539626

  Radelet, M. L., Bedau, H. A., & Putnam, C. E. (1994). In spite of innocence: Erroneous

  convictions in capital cases. Boston: Northeastern University Press.

  Rankin, P. J., & Philip, P. J. (1963). “An epidemic of laughing in the Bukoba district

  of Tanganyika.” Central African Journal of Medicine, 9(5), 167–170.

  Raskin, D. C., Honts, C. R., & Kircher, J. C. (2013). Credibility assessment: Scientific

  research and applications. London: Academic Press.

  Ratcliffe, J. M., Fenton, M. B., & Galef, B. G., Jr. (2003). “An exception to the rule:

  Common vampire bats do not learn taste aversions.” Animal Behaviour, 65(2),

  385–389.

  Reed, L. I., DeScioli, P., & Pinker, S. A. (2014). “The commitment function of angry

  facial expressions.” Psychological Science, 25(8), 1511–1517.

  Reicher, S. D. (1996). “ ‘The Crowd’ century: Reconciling practical success with theo-

  retical failure.” British Journal of Social Psy chol ogy, 35(4), 535–553.

  Reicher, S. D., Haslam, S. A., & Smith, J. R. (2012). “Working toward the experimenter:

  Reconceptualizing obedience within the Milgram paradigm as identification- based

  followership.” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4), 315–324.

  Reid, T. (1970). Inquiry into the human mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  (Original work published 1764.)

  Reyes- Jaquez, B., & Echols, C. H. (2015). “Playing by the rules: Self- interest informa-

  tion influences children’s trust and trustworthiness in the absence of feedback.”

  Cognition, 134, 140–154.

  Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone. Chicago: University of Chi-

  cago Press.

  Richter, T., Schroeder, S., & Wöhrmann, B. (2009). “You don’t have to believe every-

  thing you read: Background knowledge permits fast and efficient validation of

  information.” Journal of Personality and Social Psy chol ogy, 96(3), 538–558.

  Robbins, T. (1988). Cults, converts and charisma: The sociology of new religious move-

  ments. New York: Sage.

  Roberts, M. E. (2018). Censored: Distraction and diversion inside China’s great firewall.

  Prince ton, NJ: Prince ton University Press.

  Robertson, R. E., Jiang, S., Joseph, K., Friedland, L., Lazer, D., & Wilson, C. (2018).

  “Auditing partisan audience bias within Google search.” Proceedings of the ACM

  on Human- Computer Interaction, 2(CSCW). Retrieved from https:// dl .acm .org

  / citation .cfm ? id = 3274417

  Robertson, T. E., Sznycer, D., Delton, A. W., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2018). “The

  true trigger of shame: Social devaluation is sufficient, wrongdoing is unnecessary.”

  Evolution and Human Be hav ior, 39(5), 566–573.

  340 references

  Robinson, E. J., Champion, H., & Mitchel , P. (1999). “ Children’s ability to infer ut-

  terance veracity from speaker informedness.” Developmental Psy chol ogy, 35(2),

  535–546.

  Robinson, F. G. (1988). “The characterization of Jim in Huckleberry Finn.” Nineteenth-

  Century Lit er a ture, 43(3), 361–391.

  Robisheaux, T. W. (2009). The last witch of Langenburg: Murder in a German village.

  New York: Norton.

  Rocher, L. (1964). “The theory of proof in ancient Hindu law.” Recueil de La Société

  Jean Bodin, 18, 325–371.

  Rogers, T., & Nickerson, D. (2013). Can inaccurate beliefs about incumbents be changed?

  And can reframing change votes? Retrieved from https:// papers .ssrn .com / sol3

  / papers .cfm ? abstract _ id = 2271654

  Rose, R., Mishler, W. T., & Munro, N. (2011). Popu lar support for an undemo cratic re-

  gime: The changing views of Rus sians. https:// doi .org / 10 .1017 / CBO9780511809200

  Rosnow, R. L. (1991). “Inside rumor: A personal journey.” American Psychologist, 46(5),

  484–496.

  Rothbard, M. N. (2003). The ethics of liberty. New York: NYU Press.

  Roulin, N., & Ternes, M. (2019). “Is it time to kill the detection wizard? Emotional

  intel igence does not facilitate deception detection.” Personality and Individual Dif-

  ferences, 137, 131–138.

  Rousseau, J.- J. (2002). The social contract: And, the first and second discourses (G. May,

  Trans.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Roy, O. (2016). Le djihad et la mort. Paris: Le Seuil.

  Royed, T. J. (1996). “Testing the mandate model in Britain and the United States: Evi-

  dence from the Reagan and Thatcher eras.” British Journal of Po liti cal Science,

  26(1), 45–80.

  Rozin, P. (1976). “The se lection of foods by rats, humans, and other animals.” In R. A.


  Rosenblatt, A. Hind, E. Shaw, & C. Beer (Eds.), Advances in the study of be hav ior

  (Vol. 6, pp. 21–76). New York: Academic Press.

  Rudé, G. (1959). The crowd in the French Revolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  Rumsey, A., & Niles, D. (Eds.). (2011). Sung tales from the Papua New Guinea highlands:

  Studies in form, meaning, and sociocultural context. Camberra: ANU E Press.

  Sadler, O., & Tesser, A. (1973). “Some effects of salience and time upon interpersonal

  hostility and attraction during social isolation.” Sociometry, 36(1), 99–112.

  Safra, L., Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (submitted). Why would anyone elect an un-

  trustworthy and narcissistic leader.

  Sala, G., Aksayli, N. D., Tatlidil, K. S., Tatsumi, T., Gondo, Y., & Gobet, F. (2018). “Near

  and far transfer in cognitive training: A second- order meta- analysis.” Col abra: Psy-

  chol ogy, 5, 18. DOI: doi:10.1525/col abra.203

  r e f e r e n c e s 341

  Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). “Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess,

  music, and working memory training.” Current Directions in Psychological Science,

  26(6), 515–520.

  Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2018). “Cognitive training does not enhance general cognition.”

  Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(1), 9–20.

  Sally, D. (1995). “Conversation and cooperation in social dilemmas.” Rationality and

  Society, 7(1), 58–92.

  Salter, S. (1983). “Structures of consensus and coercion: Workers’ morale and the main-

  tenance of work discipline, 1939–1945.” In D. Welch (Ed.), Nazi propaganda: The

  power and the limitations (pp. 88–116). London: Croom Helm.

  San Roque, L., & Loughnane, R. (2012). “The New Guinea Highlands evidentiality

  area.” Linguistic Typology, 16(1), 111–167.

  Schieffelin, B. B. (1995). “Creating evidence.” Pragmatics: Quarterly Publication of the

  International Pragmatics Association, 5(2), 225–243.

  Schniter, E., Sheremeta, R. M., & Sznycer, D. (2013). “Building and rebuilding trust

  with promises and apologies.” Journal of Economic Be hav ior and Organ ization, 94,

  242–256.

  Schroeder, E., & Stone, D. F. (2015). Fox News and po liti cal knowledge. Journal of

  Public Economics, 126, 52–63.

  Schultz, D. P. (1964). Panic be hav ior: Discussion and readings (Vol. 28). New York: Ran-

  dom House.

  Schweingruber, D., & Wohlstein, R. T. (2005). “The madding crowd goes to school:

  Myths about crowds in introductory sociology textbooks.” Teaching Sociology,

  33(2), 136–153.

  Scott, J. C. (1990). Domination and the arts of re sis tance: Hidden transcripts. New Haven,

  CT: Yale University Press.

  Scott, J. C. (2008). Weapons of the weak: Everyday forms of peasant re sis tance. New

  Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  Scott- Phil ips, T. C. (2008). “Defining biological communication.” Journal of Evolu-

  tionary Biology, 21(2), 387–395.

  Scott- Phil ips, T. C. (2014). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is diff er-

  ent, and how language evolved to make it special. London: Palgrave Macmil an.

  Scott- Phil ips, T. C., Blythe, R. A., Gardner, A., & West, S. A. (2012). “How do com-

  munication systems emerge?” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,

  279(1735), 1943–1949.

  Seabright, P. (2004). The com pany of strangers: A natu ral history of economic life.

  Prince ton: Prince ton University Press.

  Sebestyen, V. (2009). Revolution 1989: The fall of the Soviet empire. London: Hachette

  UK.

  342 references

  Selb, P., & Munzert, S. (2018). “Examining a most likely case for strong campaign ef-

  fects: Hitler’s speeches and the rise of the Nazi Party, 1927–1933.” American Po liti-

  cal Science Review, 112(4), 1050–1066.

  Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). “Formidability and the logic of human

  anger.” Proceedings of the National Acad emy of Sciences, 106(35), 15073–15078.

  Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). “Vervet monkey alarm calls: Se-

  mantic communication in a free- ranging primate.” Animal Behaviour, 28(4),

  1070–1094.

  Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C., & Frith, C. D. (2014). “Supra-

  personal cognitive control and metacognition.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4),

  186–193.

  Shibutani, T. (1966). Improvised news: A so cio log i cal study of rumor. New York:

  Bobbs- Merril .

  Shore, J., Baek, J., & Del arocas, C. (2018). “Network structure and patterns of infor-

  mation diversity on Twitter.” MIS Quarterly, 42(3), 849–872.

  Shtulman, A. (2006). “Qualitative differences between naïve and scientific theories

  of evolution.” Cognitive Psy chol ogy, 52(2), 170–194.

  Shtulman, A. (2017). Scienceblind: Why our intuitive theories about the world are so often

  wrong. New York: Basic Books.

  Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). “Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not

  supplant earlier intuitions.” Cognition, 124(2), 209–215.

  Sighele, S. (1901). La foule criminelle: Essai de psychologie collective. Paris: Alcan.

  Signer, M. (2009). Demagogue: The fight to save democracy from its worst enemies. New

  York: Macmil an.

  Sigurdsson, J. F., & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1996). “The psychological characteristics of

  ‘false confessors’: A study among Icelandic prison inmates and juvenile offend-

  ers.” Personality and Individual Differences, 20(3), 321–329.

  Silver, B. (1987). “Po liti cal beliefs of the Soviet citizen: Sources of support for regime

  norms.” In J. R. Mil ar (Ed.), Politics, work, and daily life in the USSR. New York:

  Cambridge University Press.

  Silver, I., & Shaw, A. (2018). “No harm, still foul: Concerns about reputation drive

  dislike of harmless plagiarizers.” Cognitive Science, 42(S1), 213–240.

  Simler, K., & Hanson, R. (2017). The elephant in the brain: Hidden motives in everyday

  life. New York: Oxford University Press.

  Singh, M. (2018). “The cultural evolution of shamanism.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences,

  41, e66.

  Sinha, D. (1952). “Behaviour in a catastrophic situation: A psychological study of re-

  ports and rumours.” British Journal of Psy chol ogy. General Section, 43(3),

  200–209.

  r e f e r e n c e s 343

  Sklar, A. Y., Levy, N., Goldstein, A., Mandel, R., Maril, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2012).

  “Reading and doing arithmetic nonconsciously.” Proceedings of the National Acad-

  emy of Sciences, 109(48), 19614–19619.

  Smith, E. A., & Bird, R.L.B. (2000). “Turtle hunting and tombstone opening:

  Public generosity as costly signaling.” Evolution and Human Be hav ior, 21(4),

  245–261.

  Smith, M. J., Ellenberg, S. S., Bel , L. M., & Rubin, D. M. (2008). “Media coverage of

  the measles- mumps- rubel a vaccine and autism controversy and its relationship

  to MMR immunization rates in the United States.” Pediatrics, 121(4), e836– e843.

  Sniezek, J. A., Schrah, G. E., & Dalal, R. S. (2004). “Improving judgement with pre-

  paid expert advice.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(3), 173–190.

  Snow, David A., & Phil ips, C. L. (1980). “The Lofland- Stark conversion model: A criti-

  cal reassessment.�
� Social Prob lems, 27(4), 430–447.

  Snyder, J. M., & Strömberg, D. (2010). “Press coverage and po liti cal accountability.”

  Journal of Po liti cal Economy, 118(2), 355–408.

  Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Dietrich, N. (2006). “Two- to four- year- old children’s dif-

  ferentiation of knowing and guessing in a non- verbal task.” Eu ro pean Journal of

  Developmental Psy chol ogy, 3(3), 222–237.

  Sokal, A. D., & Bricmont, J. (1998). Intellectual impostures: Postmodern phi los o phers’

  abuse of science. London: Profile Books.

  Sommer, C. (2011). “Alarm calling and sentinel behaviour in Arabian babblers.” Bio-

  acoustics, 20(3), 357–368.

  Sperber, D. (1975). Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  Sperber, D. (1994). “The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of repre sen-

  ta tions.” In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain

  specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 39–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University

  Press.

  Sperber, D. (1997). “Intuitive and reflective beliefs.” Mind and Language, 12(1), 67–83.

  Sperber, D. (2010). “The guru effect.” Review of Philosophy and Psy chol ogy, 1(4),

  583–592.

  Sperber, D., & Baumard, N. (2012). “Moral reputation: An evolutionary and cogni-

  tive perspective.” Mind and Language, 27(5), 495–518.

  Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson,

  D. (2010). “Epistemic vigilance.” Mind and Language, 25(4), 359–393.

  Sperber, D., & Mercier, H. (2018). “Why a modular approach to reason?” Mind and

  Language, 131(4), 496–501.

  Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. New York:

  Wiley- Blackwel .

  Stanley, J. (2015). How propaganda works. New York: Prince ton University Press.

  344 references

  Stapleton, T. J. (1991). “ ‘They no longer care for their chiefs’: Another look at the Xhosa

  cattle- kil ing of 1856–1857.” International Journal of African Historical Studies, 24(2),

  383–392.

  Stark, R. (1984). The rise of a new world faith. Review of Religious Research, 26(1),

  18–27.

  Stark, R. (1996). The rise of Chris tian ity: A sociologist reconsiders history. Prince ton, NJ:

  Prince ton University Press.

  Stark, R. (1999). “Secularization, RIP.” Sociology of Religion, 60(3), 249–273.

 

‹ Prev