Book Read Free

The Lives of Bees

Page 16

by Thomas D Seeley


  side. Next, the raised edges of these cells are sculpted into thin lines to

  form the bases of each cell’s six walls, with the adjoining walls laid out at

  an angle of 120 degrees. This gives each cell a hexagonal cross section from

  the start. As additional bits of wax are deposited, the bases of additional

  cells begin to take shape at the appropriate distances from the preexisting

  cells, and the walls of the first cells are raised by adding rough particles of

  wax to the top of each wall and then shaving each one down on both sides

  to form a thin, smooth, plane of wax in the middle (Fig. 5.10, middle). The

  cutaway wax is then piled up, together with some fresh wax, on the top of

  the wall, and the process is repeated. So, bit by bit, a wonderfully thin wall

  of wax grows steadily outward, always crowned by a broad coping.

  Throughout this process of comb construction there runs a theme of

  economy in the use of the energetically expensive beeswax. The most

  conspicuous expression of this frugality is the cell shape in the combs of

  honey bees: a right hexagonal prism capped on the inner end by a trihedral

  pyramid. Because the cells of honey bee nests were originally circular in

  Seeley.indb 124

  2/21/2019 8:07:36 AM

  The Nest 125

  0

  10 mm

  0

  0.5 mm

  Fig. 5.10. Left: Illustration of the economy achieved by building hexagonal rather

  than circular cells in wax. Middle: Cross section through a comb showing the way

  bees precisely plane down cell walls during construction to minimize cost of

  comb building. Right: Cross sections of the outer margins of cell walls built by

  normal bees (top) and by bees from which the six distal segments of their anten-

  nae were amputated (bottom). Normally, loose bits of wax are packed together

  at the tip of the cell wall while the rest of the wall is a single, thin layer of wax.

  Bees with antennal operations built costly, triple- layered cell walls.

  cross section, as they still are in the nests of all other bees, one can think

  of the comb in a honey bee nest as an assemblage of identical cylinders

  compressed into hexagonal prisms. The perimeter of a hexagon of a given

  area is 5 percent longer than the perimeter of a circle with the same area,

  but because each hexagonal cell in a comb shares its walls with other cells,

  whereas circular cells do not, building the cell walls in a comb with hex-

  agonal cells requires only about 52 percent of the wax needed to build

  those in a comb with circular cells. For example, the average wall- to- wall

  distance of the worker cells in the nests of the wild colonies that I dissected

  was 5.20 millimeters (0.20 inches). A hexagon of this size has an area of

  Seeley.indb 125

  2/21/2019 8:07:36 AM

  126 Chapter 5

  23.40 square millimeters (0.04 square inches) and a perimeter of 18.01

  millimeters (0.71 inch). A circle with the same area has a perimeter of only

  17.15 millimeters (0.67 inch). But because each cell wall in a hexagonal-

  cell comb is shared by two cells, the effective perimeter per hexagonal cell

  is just 18.01 millimeters divided by two, which equals 9.00 millimeters

  (0.35 inch), and 9.00 millimeters divided by 17.15 millimeters equals

  0.52, hence the figure of 52 percent mentioned above. A comb of circular

  cells also requires wax to fill the spaces between the cell walls (Fig. 5.10,

  left), so the volume of wax required to build a hexagonal- cell comb with

  a given number of cells is, all things considered, less than half that required

  to build a circular- cell comb with the same number of cells.

  Several other features of the comb construction process, beside the

  hexagonal cell design, also contribute to economy in wax use by honey bee

  colonies. One is the skill of worker bees in shaving down the wax parti-

  tions between cells, which leaves the walls of the cells only 0.073 millime-

  ter (0.003 inch) thick. An experimental investigation of the sensory abili-

  ties of worker honey bees has revealed that a worker’s antennae play a

  critical role in the process of sensing cell- wall thickness. When the six

  distal segments of both antennae of several hundred bees were amputated,

  and their comb building was studied, it was found that their building prac-

  tices were grossly disrupted. Some cells were built with holes gnawed in

  their walls, while other cells were built with walls twice as thick as normal

  (Fig. 5.10, right). Because the temperature and composition of the bees’

  building material (beeswax) are constant, and because the shape of the

  cells in their combs is uniform, it is likely that a worker bee can judge the

  thickness of a cell wall by pressing on it with her mandibles and noting the

  elastic resiliency of the substrate with her antennae.

  Bees achieve further economy in wax production by constantly recy-

  cling old wax. Whenever a bee emerges as an adult, the fragments of the

  cap of her brood cell are carefully bitten off, by the emerging bee or

  nearby nurse bees, and then are stuck to the cell’s rim for reuse later.

  Likewise, queen cells—the special, large, peanut- shaped cells in which

  queen bees are reared—are built from bits of wax cut from adjoining

  Seeley.indb 126

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  The Nest 127

  worker cells, and once vacated they are torn down to free wax for other

  purposes.

  Perhaps the most important way that a honey bee colony achieves econ-

  omy in wax use is by carefully timing its comb building, limiting it to pe-

  riods when the benefits of comb construction outweigh the costs of wax

  production. Consider first the situation of a swarm that has just moved

  into an empty tree hollow. Here the benefits of comb building are huge,

  for comb is vital to the colony’s future. A fledgling colony cannot begin to

  rear brood or store food until it has built some combs, so it makes sense

  that it invests in a burst of comb building. This phenomenon was described

  recently by one of my PhD students, Michael L. Smith, who monitored

  the lives of several honey bee colonies from the moment they occupied

  their nest cavities to when they died. Each colony started out as an artifi-

  cial swarm of average size—some 12,000 worker bees plus a queen—and

  was installed in a large, glass- walled observation hive that provided a 38-

  liter (10- gallon) nest cavity. Each swarm was fed sugar syrup ad libitum

  before it was installed in its hive, so its members were stuffed with energy-

  rich food when they moved into their new homesite, as is usually the case

  for natural swarms. For the next 20 months, the three colonies were left

  undisturbed, except that once a week, at night, the insulation boards cov-

  ering the glass walls of each hive were removed to census the colony’s

  worker population and drone population; trace the area of its worker

  comb and drone comb; and record which comb cells contained brood,

  pollen, honey, or nothing at all.

  Figure 5.11 shows how each colony quickly built, over the first three

  weeks, 2,000–4,000 square centimeters (ca. 300–600 square inches) of

  comb surface
. This much comb comprises only 8,500 to 17,000 worker

  cells—less than 20 percent of the number found in a completed nest—but

  it was enough for each colony to begin rearing worker bees within a few

  days of moving into its new home. Doing so was critically important,

  because the development period of a worker honey bee is 21 days, which

  means that for the first three weeks after moving into its new home, a

  colony’s population shrinks steadily, as old workers are dying off and

  Seeley.indb 127

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  128 Chapter 5

  worker

  drone

  12

  Colony 1

  8

  ) 4

  2

  0

  s of cm 12 Colony 2

  8

  4

  0

  Fig. 5.11. Top: Histories of comb

  building in three unmanaged col-

  Comb area (1000’ 12

  Colony 3

  onies, each of which began as a

  8

  12,000- bee swarm in July 2012.

  4

  Magenta line, worker comb; blue

  0

  line, drone comb. Each colony had

  J A S O N D J F M A M J J A S

  just two pulses of comb building in

  2012

  2013

  the first year: one when it moved

  15

  Colony 1

  into its empty hive and one in late

  10

  4

  August and early September, when

  5

  2 Number of drones (1000’

  goldenrod ( Solidago spp.) plants

  s) 0

  0

  were producing copious nectar.

  15

  Colony 2

  Comb areas shown represent both

  10

  4

  sides of each comb. Bottom: Popu-

  5

  2

  lation dynamics in the same three

  0

  0

  colonies. Magenta line, workers;

  blue line, drones. The casting of a

  15

  Colony 3

  s)

  swarm (or an afterswarm) is marked

  Number of workers (1000’ 10

  4

  by an asterisk (or two). Arrow in

  5

  2

  colony 2 plot marks when colony

  0

  0

  J A S O N D J F M A M J J A S

  was given a queen to replace its

  2012

  2013

  original queen, which had died.

  young ones are not yet emerging to replenish the workforce. Indeed, Fig-

  ure 5.11 shows that by the end of the first three weeks, each colony’s

  worker population had fallen to just 20–50 percent of its original level,

  and that even by summer’s end none of the colonies had regained its origi-

  nal workforce size.

  Seeley.indb 128

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  The Nest 129

  After its initial burst of comb building to initiate brood rearing and food

  storage, a colony faces a dilemma with respect to further comb building.

  On the one hand, building more comb can help a colony to efficiently

  exploit unpredictable nectar flows by giving the colony spare storage

  space, which helps keep the nectar- receiver bees from becoming mired in

  long searches for empty cells in which to deposit the fresh nectar. On the

  other hand, building more comb can shrink a colony’s honey stores through

  the costly production of wax. Back in the 1990s, another one of my PhD

  students, Stephen C. Pratt, decided to investigate how the bees deal with

  this dilemma. Using a technique called stochastic dynamic programming,

  he started his work by modeling the situation that a colony faces in decid-

  ing when, and how much, to invest in comb building. His goal was to de-

  termine the optimal timing of additional comb construction during a

  colony’s first summer based on the availability of nectar to be collected in

  the field, the amount of nectar already stored in the comb (as honey), and

  the amount of comb already present in the nest. Stephen’s modeling work

  revealed that an established colony will achieve nearly optimal timing of

  comb building by limiting the construction of additional comb to times

  when two requirements are met: 1) the colony has filled its comb above a

  threshold level, and 2) the colony is busy collecting more nectar.

  Stephen tested this prediction by performing experiments in which he

  monitored the comb building by colonies living in three- frame observation

  hives in which one frame was filled with brood, one frame was partially

  filled with honey, and one frame was empty (to provide space for comb

  building). He moved each study colony (one at a time) to a location with-

  out natural nectar sources, so that he could control its rate of nectar intake

  by adjusting the availability of “nectar” (sugar water) from a feeder. Figure

  5.12 shows the results of one experiment that tested the prediction that a

  colony that is collecting nectar starts to build additional comb only after a

  threshold level of comb fullness has been reached. In phase 1, the colony

  had a high rate of nectar intake, but its comb fullness was kept at a low

  level. In phase 2, the high rate of nectar intake continued, and now the

  colony could fill its storage comb with honey. Finally, in phase 3, the colony

  Seeley.indb 129

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  130 Chapter 5

  ) 100

  1

  2

  3

  80

  Cells filled (% 60

  Fig. 5.12. Results of an experiment

  that tested the role of comb fullness

  ) 300

  in a colony’s decision to start build-

  ing additional comb. Phase 1: Col-

  200

  ony had a high rate of “nectar” in-

  take (collection of a 65% sucrose

  100

  solution from a feeder), but its

  Nectar intake (ml

  honey- storage comb was kept at a

  50

  low level of fullness. Phase 2: The

  2 )

  “nectar” intake continued, but now

  the bees were permitted to fill their

  30

  storage comb with honey. Phase 3:

  Colony was returned to the condi-

  Comb built (cm 10

  tions of phase 1. The search time

  120

  variable plotted at the bottom is the

  90

  time taken by a returning nectar

  60

  forager to find a food- storer bee

  time (s)

  willing to receive her nectar load.

  Mean search

  30

  The three mean search times indi-

  1

  3

  5

  7

  9

  11

  cated by dark bars are significantly

  Day

  higher than the others.

  was returned to the conditions in phase 1. As predicted, the colony built

  no comb in phase 1 but then started to do so in phase 2 once its comb full-

  ness had reached a threshold level (about 80%). In phase 3, however, the

  colony did not cease construction even though the fullness of its storage

  comb had been reduced (by Stephen)
to a low level. This experiment shows

  that colonies will start building comb before they have filled all the existing

  storage comb in their nest. I suspect that the bees follow this strategy—

  which entails some risk of premature comb construction—to be sure that

  they have adequate storage space in the event of a large honey flow. This

  Seeley.indb 130

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  The Nest 131

  experiment also shows that once a colony has begun building comb, it will

  continue doing this so long as it enjoys a strong influx of nectar and thus a

  good supply of the fuel for comb building. Stephen also performed experi-

  ments in which colonies were deprived of a high rate of nectar intake, but

  their storage comb had a high level of fullness (85% or more). He found

  that these colonies did not build comb. Taken together, these two experi-

  ments show that Stephen’s modeling analysis was correct; a colony invests

  in additional comb building only when it has reached a threshold level of

  comb fullness and it is experiencing a strong influx of nectar.

  How do worker bees monitor both the comb fullness and the nectar

  intake rate of their colony so that they know when to start comb building

  at the right time? One possibility is that when a colony’s storage combs get

  close to being full, the nectar receivers—the bees that unload the return-

  ing nectar foragers—experience increasing difficulty in finding cells for

  storing the fresh nectar, and they respond to this coincidence of high nec-

  tar influx and increasing comb fullness by starting to secrete wax and build

  comb. In support of this hypothesis is our knowledge that the nectar re-

  ceivers and the comb builders are both middle- aged bees, about 10–20

  days old, which means that nectar receivers are the right age to become

  comb builders. Further support of this hypothesis comes from the experi-

  ment presented in Figure 5.12. In performing this experiment, Stephen

  noticed that the average search time of a nectar forager—the time she spent

  searching in the hive for a bee who would receive her nectar load—rose

  markedly when the colony’s comb construction started up. This conspicu-

  ous rise in the search times of the nectar foragers may have been caused

  by many of the colony’s middle- aged bees switching from nectar receiving

  to comb building. However, when Stephen applied paint marks to 30–40

  percent of the nectar receivers in a colony, and then induced this colony

  to start building comb, he found that the marked bees made up less than

 

‹ Prev