Book Read Free

The Lives of Bees

Page 17

by Thomas D Seeley


  5 percent of the comb builders. This surprising result shows that future

  work on the control of comb building needs to focus on the behavior of

  unemployed comb builders, to find out how these bees acquire information

  about their colony’s comb fullness and nectar intake.

  Seeley.indb 131

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  132 Chapter 5

  CONTROL OF COMB TYPE

  Besides deciding when to build comb, colonies must decide what type to

  build, worker comb or drone comb. Both types of comb are used for honey

  storage, but only the large cells in drone comb can be used for rearing the

  large, heavy- bodied drones. A colony living in the wild devotes a surpris-

  ingly high percentage of the total comb area in its nest to drone comb: 17

  percent, on average (Fig. 5.13). It does so because having plenty of drone

  comb enables a colony to invest heavily in producing drones, which are

  critical to its reproductive (genetic) success. Figure 5.11, from Michael L.

  Smith’s study, shows, however, that during the first several weeks after

  moving into their new homesites, all three colonies in this study built only

  worker cells and reared only worker bees. This makes sense, for it is

  worker bees that will build the combs and collect the honey and pollen

  that are essential for the survival of a fledgling colony.

  Eventually, however, a healthy colony will grow strong enough to invest

  in building both drone comb and worker comb. How does it regulate the

  proportion of each comb type in its nest? In principle, each comb- builder

  bee—or perhaps the queen—must be informed about the current relative

  amount of each kind of comb in the nest, but how is this possible given the

  large size of the whole nest relative to that of a worker bee? The queen is

  uniquely suited to evaluate this feature because she spends much of her

  time—when she is not resting or being groomed—walking around on the

  comb and measuring the sizes of empty cells to decide which kind of egg

  to lay: fertilized eggs in worker cells and unfertilized ones in drone cells.

  If she keeps a running count of the two cell types, then she might be able

  to perceive any imbalance between the two types of comb and might com-

  municate this to the workers so they can correct it by adjusting the type

  of new comb they build.

  Stephen C. Pratt investigated the information pathways that the bees use

  to guide their decisions about which type of comb to build. He did so by

  setting up queenright, full- size colonies housed in two 10- frame hive bod-

  ies and then adjusting the number and type of combs in each hive and the

  Seeley.indb 132

  2/21/2019 8:07:37 AM

  The Nest 133

  4

  2

  Number of nests 0

  0

  5

  10

  15

  20

  25

  Drone comb (%)

  Fig. 5.13. Top: The cell diameters in natural honey comb have a bimodal distribu-

  tion: the smaller size on the left (approximately 5.2 millimeters/0.20 inches wall

  to wall) is used to rear worker brood, and the larger size on the right (approxi-

  mately 6.5 millimeters/0.26 inches wall to wall) is used to rear drone brood.

  Bottom: The percentage of drone comb by area in the nests of 8 wild colonies

  collected in upstate New York, and 22 simulated wild colonies. Values are clus-

  tered around the mean value of 17 percent.

  access by the workers to the combs in their hive, to create different experi-

  mental treatments (Fig. 5.14). Stephen’s experiments revealed three things.

  First, the bees in a colony require contact with the drone comb for its

  presence to inhibit their building of additional drone comb. Second, con-

  tact with the drone comb by the queen plays no part in this inhibition. Third,

  the inhibition of building more drone comb can come from the drone

  Seeley.indb 133

  2/21/2019 8:07:38 AM

  134 Chapter 5

  Drone comb

  No drone comb

  Empty frame

  Worker comb

  Drone comb

  Fig. 5.14. Design of the experiment testing the need of the bees to have direct

  contact with drone comb for proper regulation of drone- comb building. Bees

  (workers and queen) were separated from two drone combs by a wire- mesh

  screen in the Drone Comb treatment. As a control, two worker combs were

  similarly screened off in the No Drone Comb treatment. In both treatments, the

  hive had two empty frames where the bees built comb. A feeder jar filled with

  sugar syrup (to encourage comb building) sits atop each hive.

  comb itself, though the inhibition is even stronger if the drone comb is

  filled with brood. These findings show that the workers are not responding

  to a volatile chemical signal built into the drone comb, since they require

  direct contact to respond to its presence. These findings also show that the

  queen is not acting as a central regulator of drone- comb building, even

  though she is well positioned to collect information on drone comb need.

  So how do workers decide which type of comb to build? It may be that each

  comb- builder bee crawls over the existing combs and slowly accumulates

  information on their cell- size composition by measuring directly some

  number of cells. And it may be that worker bees use the same mechanism

  of cell- size measurement as is used by queen bees: inserting the head and

  forelegs into a cell and sensing how they contact the cell’s walls.

  To summarize, what we now know about how bees control the type of

  cells they build is that a comb- builder bee may be informed about the need

  Seeley.indb 134

  2/21/2019 8:07:38 AM

  The Nest 135

  for each type of cell by noting the relative encounter rates that she has had

  with completed worker and drone cells. We also know that a comb- builder

  bee may be able to coordinate her work—that is, whether to build worker

  comb or drone comb—with that of her nest mates by noting the relative

  encounter rates that she has had with cells under construction. Each of these

  hypotheses, however, awaits a rigorous experimental test.

  THE PROPOLIS ENVELOPE

  Perhaps the most eye- catching difference between the inside of a hollow-

  tree home of a wild colony and the inside of a lumber- built hive of a

  managed colony is the look of their walls. The walls inside a bee tree are

  coated with tree resins that make them shiny and waterproof (Fig. 5.4),

  whereas the walls inside a bee hive—even one used for years and years—

  have no such coating, so they usually look as dull and porous as freshly

  planed boards. In short, only wild colonies live surrounded by a propolis

  envelope. Most of this envelope is a thin (less than 1 millimeter/0.04

  inch) coating of resin on the nest cavity’s walls, but if there are cracks in

  the walls the bees will fill them with dark seams of resin. Also, if the en-

  trance opening is oversize—hence too drafty or too inviting to invad-

  ers—the bees will reduce it by building a sturdy wall of dried resin (Fig.

  5.7). Constricting the entrance opening is the most conspicuous use of

&nb
sp; tree resins by the bees, and it is the origin of the word beekeepers use for

  the resins bees apply in their hives, propolis ( pro: in front of; polis: city or community).

  What are the bees’ sources of tree resins, and how do the bees collect

  this sticky stuff? The only tree on which I have observed bees collecting

  resin (from sticky leaf buds) is an eastern cottonwood tree ( Populus deltoi-

  des) that grows near my laboratory. However, bees have been observed by

  others collecting resin from the buds and wounds of many other tree spe-

  cies in North America and Europe, especially horse chestnut ( Aesculus hip-

  pocastanum), white poplar ( Populus alba), quaking aspens ( Populus tremula

  and P. tremuloides), and various birches ( Betula spp.). In these species, the

  leaf buds shine with protective coatings of resin. Bees further collect resin

  Seeley.indb 135

  2/21/2019 8:07:38 AM

  136 Chapter 5

  Fig. 5.15. Honey bee with a load of shiny resin in the corbicula (pollen basket)

  of her left hind leg.

  from conifers—including spruces ( Picea spp.), pines ( Pinus spp.), and

  larches ( Larix spp.)—that have injury sites oozing the trees’ protective

  resins. A resin- collector bee loads herself with this gluey material by chew-

  ing off a bit of resin with the mandibles, then grasping it with the forelegs,

  passing it to one of her midlegs, and finally shifting it from the midleg to

  the corbicula (pollen basket) on the same side. This action is repeated until

  a load of glistening resin fills each corbicula (Fig. 5.15). When a resin col-

  lector returns to her nest, she crawls across the combs to one of the places

  where resin is being used and then she stands there for 5–20 minutes while

  other bees, resin users, bite off pieces of resin from the two loads in her

  corbiculae.

  The resin collectors and resin users of a honey bee colony are fascinat-

  ing bees. But who are they? How do they handle resin inside their nests?

  And how do they control their collection of this building material? Until

  recently, we had no solid answers to these questions. I was delighted,

  therefore, when Professor Jun Nakamura, from the Honeybee Science

  Seeley.indb 136

  2/21/2019 8:07:38 AM

  The Nest 137

  Research Center at Tamagawa University in Japan, came to my laboratory,

  in 2002, to try to solve these mysteries. He conducted his investigations

  using a colony of approximately 3,000 bees that lived in a glass- walled

  observation hive that was housed in a heated room. To study individual

  resin users and resin collectors, he added eight cohorts of zero- day- old bees

  to the colony, one cohort every three to four days, during the month of

  May. He quickly learned how rare the resin users can be, for he observed

  only 10 of his 800 labeled bees engaged in resin- use behaviors: caulking

  (forcing resin into cracks and crevices) and resin taking (biting resin from

  the corbiculae of a resin collector). He also learned that all 10 resin users

  were middle- aged bees. They were doing their resin work when they were

  14–24 days old, which was after they had quit working as nurse bees (e.g.,

  feeding larvae and eating pollen) and before they would begin working as

  foragers. The resin collectors in Jun’s colony were all elderly bees (age

  range 25–38 days old), as were the nectar, water, and pollen collectors in

  this colony.

  Jun also learned that with resin—unlike with pollen, nectar, or water—

  there is not a strict partitioning of work between collectors working out-

  side the hive and users working inside the hive. This became clear when he

  video- recorded 35 resin collectors, starting when each one entered the

  observation hive. He found that upon entering, each resin collector walked

  quickly and directly to a place in the top or side of the hive where resin

  users were getting resin from collectors and using it to fill the cracks be-

  tween the hive’s glass walls and its wooden frame (Fig. 5.16). Five of these

  resin collectors had carried home a chunk of resin in their mouthparts—

  along with full loads in their corbiculae—and they walked immediately to

  a work site and began to do some caulking themselves, using the resin they

  carried in their mandibles. In contrast, the resin collectors whose loads

  were entirely in their corbiculae did not engage in caulking. They simply

  stood around for 5–18 minutes, waiting for bees (resin users) to bite off

  chunks of their loads and carry them off to sites being caulked.

  It is still not known how the resin collectors sensed their colony’s need

  for resin. They could have done this directly (while engaged in using resin,

  Seeley.indb 137

  2/21/2019 8:07:39 AM

  138 Chapter 5

  Resin

  Nectar

  ggle-dancing

  Wa

  g

  Unloadin

  Fig. 5.16. Spatial distributions of waggle dancing and unloading for resin collec-

  tors and nectar foragers of a colony living in a two- frame observation hive. Arrow

  at bottom of each plot denotes the hive entrance.

  if they did so) or indirectly (by noting their unloading delays) or both. It

  seems clear that the resin collectors in the study colony sensed an ongoing

  need for resin, because of the 102 resin collectors that Jun labeled and

  watched come and go from his observation hive, 68 (67%) continued col-

  lecting resin for the rest of their lives. (The other 34 bees eventually

  switched to collecting nectar or pollen.) It may be that these resin collec-

  tors sensed an ongoing need for resin, because every week or so Jun re-

  placed the dirty glass walls on his observation hive with clean ones, and in

  doing so he probably stimulated the resin users by reopening the crevices

  where the glass walls contacted the wooden frame of the hive. We know

  that the resin collectors are stimulated by finding gaps, crevices, and rough

  surfaces—all places that are hard to keep clean—in their nest cavities.

  (Indeed, many of the bee- supply companies in the United States now sell

  propolis traps: plastic sheets that are perforated with hundreds of narrow

  Seeley.indb 138

  2/21/2019 8:07:39 AM

  The Nest 139

  slots about the width of a pencil’s lead. These sheets are laid across the

  combs in the topmost hive body, to stimulate the bees to collect and de-

  posit propolis, which will be harvested for use in making antibiotic tinc-

  tures and lotions.) Recently, Michael Simone- Finstrom and colleagues at

  the University of Minnesota have discovered that resin collectors are bet-

  ter than pollen collectors at learning to associate the stimulus of their

  antennae touching a surface with a 1- millimeter (0.04- inch) wide slot

  with the reward of a droplet of sugar syrup. The same is true when resin

  collectors and pollen foragers were tested for their ability to learn to as-

  sociate touching a rough surface (a small piece of sandpaper) with a sugar-

  syrup reward. It is not known whether this difference between resin col-

  lectors and pollen foragers in learning tactile stimuli is based on their

  recent experiences or their genetic
s, but either way, it is likely that this

  ability helps resin collectors to learn about the presence of gaps, crevices,

  and rough surfaces in their colony’s nest.

  Most beekeepers find propolis a messy hindrance to their work, since

  it can make it hard to crack open a hive and manipulate the frames of comb

  inside. For the bees, though, propolis must be extremely valuable, for

  otherwise they would not expend the effort of chewing off bits of sticky

  plant resins, packing them onto their hind legs, hauling the gluey blobs

  back to their hive, and then using this sticky stuff to fill the cracks and

  coat the walls inside their nest cavities. Recent work has revealed that the

  propolis envelope functions mainly as an antimicrobial surface that lowers

  a colony’s costs of defense against disease. We will examine this subject

  closely in chapter 10, when we explore the multifaceted matter of colony

  defense. But before we dive deeply into this and the three other major

  topics of colony functioning—reproduction, food collection, and thermo-

  regulation—we will look broadly at the life of a wild colony by examining

  its remarkable annual cycle.

  Seeley.indb 139

  2/21/2019 8:07:39 AM

  6

  ANNUAL CYCLE

  Oh, give us pleasure in the orchard white,

  Like nothing else by day, like ghosts by night;

  And make us happy in the happy bees,

  The swarm dilating round the perfect trees.

  —Robert Frost, “A Prayer in Spring,” 1915

  One key to understanding the natural lives of honey bee colonies living in

  cold- climate regions is their unique annual cycle. In winter, when colonies

  of all the other social bees—bumble bees and social sweat bees—have

  dwindled away, leaving only a residue of mated queens living alone deep

  in hibernation, honey bee colonies continue to function as full social

  groups, each one consisting of some 15,000 worker bees and one queen

  bee. Moreover, rather than becoming dormant, as is the rule for insects

  living in cold climates, honey bee colonies remain active and fight the cold.

  Each one keeps the perimeter temperature of its winter cluster above

  about 10°C (50°F), even in ambient temperatures of −30°C (−22°F) or

  colder. To achieve such strong temperature control, honey bees nest inside

 

‹ Prev