Book Read Free

The Lives of Bees

Page 25

by Thomas D Seeley


  within several hundred meters of the hive, but that they also flew to many

  flower patches several kilometers from their home. The modal (most com-

  mon) distance from homesite to flower patch was 0.7 kilometers (about

  0.4 miles), the median distance was 1.7 kilometers (1.1 miles), the aver-

  age distance was 2.3 kilometers (1.4 miles), and the maximum distance

  was 10.9 kilometers (6.8 miles). It was fascinating, too, to see that the

  average distance flown by the bees to reach their food sources varied across

  the summer. For example, during the second period of data collection,

  9–17 July 1980, the average distance flown was ca. 2 kilometers (1.2

  miles), but during the third period, 28 July to 5 August, the average dis-

  tance flown was ca. 5 kilometers (3 miles). Evidently, the foragers in our

  study colony had difficulty finding good food within the Arnot Forest dur-

  ing late July and early August; their dances showed us that throughout this

  time they collected most of their food by flying 5–6 kilometers (3.0–3.6

  miles) to the Pony Hollow valley to the north. We knew there were several

  farms in this valley, and a check of their fields revealed honey bees busily

  collecting nectar and pollen on alfalfa plants in bloom.

  The most noteworthy feature of the distribution shown in Figure 8.6 is

  the location of the 95th percentile, which falls at 6 kilometers (3.7 miles).

  Seeley.indb 199

  2/21/2019 8:07:52 AM

  200 Chapter 8

  200

  160

  120

  80

  Number of dances 40

  8

  6

  4

  aggling duration (s) 2

  W

  2

  4

  6

  8

  10

  Distance from hive to flowers (km)

  Fig. 8.6. Top: Distribution of distances to a colony’s forage sites, based on analysis

  of 1,871 waggle dances. Bottom: Coding of distance information in waggle dance:

  the duration of each waggle run is proportional to the length of the outbound

  flight.

  This shows that if we were to draw a circle around the hive that was large

  enough to enclose 95 percent of this colony’s food sources, then it would

  have an area greater than 113 square kilometers (43 square miles)! One

  might wonder, though, was our study colony exceptional in conducting

  such widespread foraging? I believe that the answer is no, for several rea-

  sons. The first is the biological reality that the bees’ system of recruitment

  communication—the waggle dance—has been tuned by natural selection

  so that successful foragers can guide their hive mates to rich food sources

  that are 10- plus kilometers (6 or more miles) from their home (see Fig.

  8.6). This shows us that their communication system has evolved to coor-

  dinate foraging over great distances. Second, there are several other stud-

  ies that have reported honey bees flying 10 or more kilometers from their

  Seeley.indb 200

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  Food Collection 201

  hives to capitalize on rich foraging sources. The first is the 1953 study by

  Herta Knaffl, the postdoctoral student of Karl von Frisch who pioneered

  the method of spying on the waggle dances performed in a colony’s nest

  to investigate the distances that its foragers are traveling. She reported

  that 95 percent of the 2,456 dances that she decoded in colonies living in

  small observation hives—all located within the city of Graz, Austria—

  advertised horse- chestnut trees ( Aesculus hippocastanum) and other food

  sources less than 2 kilometers (1.2 miles) away. However, she also re-

  ported observing dances that announced discoveries of bountiful food

  sources 5–6 kilometers (3.0–3.6 miles) and even 9–10 kilometers (5.4–

  6.0 miles) away.

  The most impressive demonstration of a honey bee colony’s capacity for

  long- range foraging comes from a study conducted by Madeleine Beekman

  and Francis L. W. Ratnieks, with a colony living in an observation hive lo-

  cated within the city of Sheffield, in England. They video- recorded and

  then decoded 444 waggle dances performed by their colony’s foragers on

  three days in the middle of August 1996, a time when vast patches of a

  major nectar source, ling heather ( Calluna vulgaris), were in full bloom on

  the moors in the Peak District west of Sheffield. Their data analysis revealed

  that their study colony foraged in two areas, one relatively close to the hive,

  less than 2 kilometers (1.2 miles) away, and one much farther away, 5–10

  kilometers (3.1 to 6.2 miles). Overall, 50 percent of the dances performed

  in their observation hive represented sites more than 6 kilometers (3.6

  miles) from the hive, and 10 percent represented sites more than 9.5 ki-

  lometers (5.9 miles) away! Clearly, there are times when a honey bee

  colony benefits greatly from having workers with a long maximum flight

  range, for this endows its food- collection operation with a vast scope.

  TREASURE HUNTING BY THE BEES

  To benefit fully from its immense foraging range of 6- plus kilometers (3.7-

  plus miles), a honey bee colony must be able to discover the richest flower

  patches within the ca. 100- square- kilometer (ca. 40- square- mile) region

  around its nest. Furthermore, it must be able discover these first- class food

  Seeley.indb 201

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  202 Chapter 8

  sources soon after they come into bloom, before they are dominated by

  competing colonies. These considerations raise an important question:

  How good is a wild colony’s surveillance of its environment for rich flower

  patches popping up in the swamplands, woodlands, and fields that form

  the landscape around its home? I addressed this question in the mid- 1980s

  by performing an experiment in which I conducted two treasure hunts

  with four colonies of honey bees. In both trials of this experiment, I cre-

  ated lush patches of flowering buckwheat plants, dispersed within a large

  forest, and I measured each colony’s success in finding these prize food

  sources.

  The forest was the 3,213- hectare (7,840- acre) Yale Myers Forest in

  northeastern Connecticut. It is a beautiful study site that has much in com-

  mon with the Arnot Forest in New York. Both are large wooded areas

  surrounded by state and private forestlands. Also, both have a history of

  human use, which includes abandonment from agriculture by the 1870s

  and then regrowth to a lightly managed, mixed- species hardwood forest

  that has some stands of eastern hemlock and eastern white pine trees. The

  Yale Myers Forest, however, has one habitat feature not found in the Arnot

  Forest: large, open wetlands created by beaver activity. These sunny ex-

  panses—brimming with cattail and purple loosestrife plants—provide

  much food for the bees. Before conducting this experiment, I searched the

  southeastern corner of the Yale Myers Forest and found four wild honey

  bee colonies living in hollow trees, which told me that the Yale Myers For-

  est, like the Arnot, is a prime habitat for wild colonies of Apis mellifera.

  Indeed, this is
as true now as it was in the 1980s. I had no difficulty finding

  honey bees on flowers deep in this forest when, in August 2017, I spent a

  day there bee hunting.

  The layout of my experiment consisted of four clustered hives of bees

  and six dispersed patches of buckwheat (Fig. 8.7). The buckwheat patches

  were identical in size—100 square meters (1,078 square feet)—but they

  differed in distance from the hives: 1.0–3.6 kilometers (0.6–2.2 miles). I

  timed the planting of these flower patches so they would come into bloom

  when little other forage would be available—either in late June, after the

  Seeley.indb 202

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  Food Collection 203

  2000 m

  2000

  3200 m

  1000 m

  N

  Hives

  1000 m

  1900 m

  3600 m

  Fig. 8.7. Top: One of six patches of buckwheat planted in the Yale Myers Forest

  to assess the extent of a honey bee colony’s reconnaissance for rich food sources.

  Bottom: Map of the array of experimental buckwheat patches in the forest.

  Seeley.indb 203

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  204 Chapter 8

  nectar flows from raspberry ( Rubus spp.) and sumac ( Rhus spp.), or in mid-

  August, before the nectar flow from goldenrod—hence at times when

  colonies would be eager to exploit my buckwheat flowers. Once the

  patches were fully in bloom, I went to each one, daubed paint of a patch-

  specific color on 150 of the approximately 200 bees busily foraging in each

  patch, and then scooted back to the four hives to watch each one’s en-

  trance for foragers bearing my paint marks. If I saw bees entering or leav-

  ing a hive with paint marks of a certain color, then I knew that the colony

  inhabiting this hive had discovered the corresponding patch. I learned

  from this experiment that the four colonies had high probabilities of dis-

  covering the patches at 1,000 meters/0.6 miles ( p = 0.70) and at 2,000

  meters/1.2 miles ( p = 0.50), but that—to my surprise—none found the

  patches at 3,200 and 3,600 meters/2.0 and 3.7 miles ( p = 0.00).

  I suspect that these probabilities understate the actual surveillance abil-

  ity of a honey bee colony, if only because my method for determining

  which colonies discovered each patch probably did not detect all the dis-

  coveries made by my four colonies. If a colony had only a handful of forag-

  ers visiting a flower patch, then probably I would have failed to see that it

  had found this patch. Nevertheless, my treasure- hunt experiment revealed

  an impressive ability by colonies to monitor their environment for rich

  food sources. Even though a 100- square- meter (1,100- square- foot) patch

  of flowers—about half the size of a tennis court—represents less than

  1/125,000 of the area enclosed by a circle with a 2- kilometer (1.2- mile)

  radius, my four colonies had a probability of 0.5 or higher of discovering

  any flower patch of this size located within 2 kilometers of its hive.

  CHOOSING AMONG FOOD SOURCES

  For a colony to be skilled at extracting food from the myriad flowers in the

  woods, swamps, fields, and gardens around its nest, it must couple its

  impressive ability to discover productive flower patches with an ability to

  selectively dispatch its foragers to the richest ones. In other words, it must

  be skilled not only at exploring its options, but also at choosing among

  them. Our first sign that a wild colony does indeed possess an impressive

  Seeley.indb 204

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  Food Collection 205

  collective intelligence in choosing among its broadly dispersed food

  sources emerged from the study described earlier in this chapter in which

  Kirk Visscher and I monitored the waggle dances of a full- size colony living

  in an observation hive in the middle of the Arnot Forest. As explained

  above, we took data each day from a random sample of the bees perform-

  ing waggle dances in our hive, and we plotted the locations of the food

  sources that these bees had advertised with their dances. We now know

  that it is only the bees that have visited top- quality forage sites that per-

  form long- lasting waggle dances—the ones that Kirk and I were most

  likely to watch—so we are confident that each day’s map of the colony’s

  recruitment targets revealed the whereabouts of the colony’s most attrac-

  tive food sources for the day.

  These maps, four of which are shown in Figure 8.8, revealed that the

  spatial distribution of a colony’s richest, most exciting food sources can

  change dramatically from one day to the next. Indeed, on each of our 36

  days of spying on the colony’s waggle- dance communications, we found

  that the colony’s dances revealed a new spatial distribution of its primary

  recruitment targets. The following day- by- day review summarizes these

  dynamics for the four- day period represented in Figure 8.8.

  13 June 1980. Good weather. The main foci of recruitment were

  clearly indicated: sites 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) SSE and SSW of

  the hive, producing yellow and yellow- gray pollen, and a large site

  2–4 kilometers (1.2–2.4 miles) SSW of the hive, producing mainly

  nectar. Two other sites profitable enough to elicit long- lasting dances

  were one producing orange pollen 1 kilometer (0.6 miles) to the NE

  and one producing yellow- gray pollen 4 kilometers (2.4 miles) to

  the NE.

  14 June 1980. Good weather. The source of yellow- gray pollen 4

  kilometers (2.4 miles) to the NE now aroused few, if any, dances;

  none were “hit” in our sampling of the dances. A nectar source 0.5

  kilometers (0.3 miles) to the NW became extremely profitable,

  stimulating many bees to advertise it with persistent dances. The pol-

  Seeley.indb 205

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  206 Chapter 8

  len sources 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) SSE and SSW of the hive and

  the nectar source 2–4 kilometers (1.2–2.4 miles) SSW remained

  highly attractive.

  15 June 1980. Good weather. The large nectar source 2–4 kilometers

  (1.2–2.4 miles) to the SSW has become less exciting, as has the nec-

  tar source 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) to the NW. The two sites pro-

  ducing mainly yellow and yellow- gray pollen 0.5 kilometers (0.3

  miles) to the SSE and SSW remained highly desirable. A source of

  brown pollen ca. 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) to the SW was adver-

  tised for the first time.

  16 June 1980. Cool and intermittent rain. The bees foraged relatively

  little and only rather close to the hive. The source of yellow- gray

  pollen 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) to the SSW remained attractive,

  but the nearby source of yellow pollen in the SSE stimulated no danc-

  ing. A source of orange pollen 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) to the NW

  was exciting. The richest source of nectar on this damp day was a new

  site 0.5 kilometers (0.3 miles) S of the hive.

  A more detailed picture of a colony’s ability to choose among a dynamic

  array of foraging opportunities comes from an experiment performed

  several years later, in which ins
tead of simply monitoring changes in the

  waggle dances advertising different foraging sites, I undertook the techni-

  cally greater challenge of measuring changes in the number of foragers

  engaged in exploiting different foraging sites. To accomplish this, I worked

  with a colony in which all 4,000 of its workers were painstakingly labeled

  for individual identification. After labeling these bees at my bee laboratory

  in Ithaca over a two- day period, I moved this special colony 240 kilometers

  (150 miles) north to one of my favorite study sites, the Cranberry Lake

  Biological Station (CLBS). This remote field station is in the northwestern

  corner of the 24,400- square- kilometer (9,375- square- mile) Adirondack

  Park in northern New York State. For at least 10 kilometers (6 miles) in

  all directions, the landscape around the CLBS is one of forests, ponds,

  Seeley.indb 206

  2/21/2019 8:07:53 AM

  Food Collection 207

  N

  N

  13 June

  14 June

  1

  2

  3

  4

  1

  2

  3

  4

  km

  km

  nectar

  yellow-gray pollen

  yellow pollen

  N

  orange pollen

  N

  brown pollen

  15 June

  16 June

  1

  2

  3

  4

  1

  2

  3

  4

  km

  km

  Fig. 8.8. Daily maps of a colony’s foraging sites, as inferred from reading the

  waggle dances performed by the colony’s foragers. Each dot represents the loca-

  tion indicated by one bee’s dance. Black dots denote sites yielding nectar; all

  other dots denote sites yielding pollen of the color shown. On the four days

  represented here, only a small fraction (2%) of the dances indicated foraging sites

  beyond 4 kilometers (2.4 miles), and most of these sites are not shown.

  bogs, and lakes, so it contains no rich food sources for the bees. Indeed,

  the bee forage here is so scanty that no wild colonies of honey bees live in

  the area, though there are some colonies of bumble bees. This meant that

  there was no risk of foragers from wild honey bee colonies intruding upon

  my experiment and no risk of natural food sources luring my meticulously

 

‹ Prev